Attraction Ppt

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    1/38

    AGREEMENT IN ATTRACTION

    J. Carlos Acua Faria

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    2/38

    () there are at least two different levels at whichagreement must work, calling on two different kinds ofinformation. Speakers begin with messages, whichembody the conceptual relationships they intend tocommunicate. The specific embodiments of conceptswithin messages are collectively called notions,comprising intended referents, ideas, states of affairs,and relationships among them. These notionalcomponents of messages carry features of theconcepts that they instantiate, but in order to becommunicated, they have to undergo linguistic codingas words standing in particular structural relationshipsto one another. Agreement may involve the notional

    features of messages, or the linguistic features ofwords and structures, or more likely both. (Bock et al.2001: 84)

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    3/38

    Under the assumption of a tight connection

    between grammar and processor (anassumption which is rarely made ever since

    the pioneering days of the derivational theory

    of complexity, but which seems to us to be

    the null hypothesis), the different derivational

    steps assumed in linguistics should betraceable in linguistic performance, and for

    our concerns here, in the way speakers err

    when producing agreement. (Franck, Lasso,Frauenfelder & Rizzi 2006: 179)

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    4/38

    *The key to the cabinets are in the

    kitchen

    the aberrant outcome of a normal resolutionprocess, a kind of spurious resolution between

    conflicting number specifications (Bock et. al

    2001: 85-86; cfr. Corbett 1983).

    proximity concord(Jespersen 1924; Kimball and

    Aissen 1971; Quirk et al. 1985; Francis 1986; denDikken 2001: Huddleston & Pullum 2002: 500 ff.;

    see Bock & Miller 1991 for review).

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    5/38

    (1) ?Its part of ones linguistic competence to be able to control and

    interpret variations of word-order and grammatical structure of the kindthat are exemplified in the sentences cited above. (grammatically

    judgement in original). (Huddleston & Pullum (2002: 500-01)

    (2) *The time for fun and games are over.

    (3) *The readiness of our conventional forces are at an all-time low.

    (4) *I dont think it much matters where the final reinterment of these

    men are.

    (5) *The learning skills people have entering college is less than it

    should be. (Bock & Miller 1991):

    the illiteracy level of our children are appalling (George Bush,Washington, 23 January 2004)

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    6/38

    Eberhard et al. (2005): when the complex NP

    contains a singular head and a plural modifier(as in the key to the cabinets), there are as

    many as 13% of agreement mistakes on the

    verb in English. This points to an

    architecturally-driven propensity

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    7/38

    (6a) The key to the cabinets (IS LARGE)

    (6b) The keys to the cabinet (*ARE LARGE)

    Seminal work led by Bock in the early 1990sset this experimental agenda in motion

    (Bock& Miller 1991; Bock & Cutting 1992;

    Bock, Cutting & Eberhard 1992; and Bock &

    Eberhard 1993).

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    8/38

    INITIAL FINDINGS 1. Asymmetry: [sg + pl], but not [pl + sg].

    2. Distributivity:

    The bridge to the islands vs

    The key to the cabinets

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    9/38

    INITIAL FINDINGS 3. Phrasal modifiers vs clausal modifiers: more

    att with phrases.

    (the same encoding cycle (Bock 1991; see also

    Nicol 1995).

    4. Short vs long postmodifier: no effects.

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    10/38

    INITIAL FINDINGS 5. Palpability of local noun reference:

    The idea was that the number of a relatively concrete

    local noun may hold more sway over the judged number of

    an abstract subject (as in the speech of the authors) than arelatively abstract local noun does over the judged number

    of a concrete subject (as in the mountain of the nomads)

    (Bock & Miller 1991: 66) No effects

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    11/38

    INITIAL CONCLUSIONS An inflectional account of agreement: feature

    inheritance, copying or percolation (Gazdar etal. 1985; Chomsky 1981): a controller which

    possesses inherent features passes them on

    to a target to establish an agreement relation.

    This is done in a formally encapsulated

    manner that is strongly reminiscent of cyclicphases. Meaning waits.

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    12/38

    Further evidence 1. pseudo-plurals did not affect attraction rates (so the player

    on the course did not attract (despite the local noun lookinglike a plural) but the player on the courts did);

    2. regulars (boys) and irregulars (men) attracted

    approximately the same;

    3. collectives like armyorfleetdid not attract whereas

    ordinarily-inflected nouns like soldiers or ships did (Bock &Eberhard 1993).

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    13/38

    Early corrections 1 1. Distributivity:

    First, Vigliocco, Butterworth, & Semenza (1995) showed that

    Italian attraction patterns were sensitive to the distributivity

    of the preambles. Then, Vigliocco, Butterworth, & Garrett(1996) compared English to Spanish and found that Spanish

    behaved like Italian, while English remained unaffected by

    semantics.

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    14/38

    Early corrections 1 Another theory: unification

    with rich morphology, the two constituents that participate

    in an agreement relation may specify only partial

    information about a single linguistic object (Kay 1985;Barlow 1988, 1993; Pollard & Sag 1988). Unification then

    occurs when compatible featural information on two sites

    becomes merged (Shieber 1986; De Smedt 1990). On thisview, features are not copied or moved, but simply partially

    shared.

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    15/38

    Early corrections 1 Unlike English, Spanish and Italian have verbs which contain such

    partial information directly and can therefore directly connect to

    conceptual structure without any kind of mediation or control.Hence the semantic (distributivity) effects.

    Vigliocco, Hartsuiker, Jarema, & Kolk (1996) obtained the samepattern of results in French and Dutch, also two richly-inflectedlanguages.

    Recall that a problem for directional theories of agreement is thatsometimes the targets of agreement exhibit marking even when itis absent from the source. For instance, in French one saysje suisheureuxvsje suis hereuse (I am happy) depending on the sex ofthe referent of the subject pronoun, via a direct appeal topragmatics (asje does not mark gender).

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    16/38

    Early corrections 2 Eberhard (1997): imageability in English.

    Humphreys and Bock (1999; cited in Bock et al. 2001: 87)

    found that plural verbs are used more frequently after subject

    NPs such as The gang on the motorcycles than after otherssuch as The gang near the motorcycles. This is because the

    former puts gang members into a one-to-one relationship

    with motorcycles, thereby emphasizing their multiplicity.

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    17/38

    Alos the initial conclusions about form

    biasses had to be revised

    Franck, Vigliocco, & Nicol (2002) on NPs

    containing three nouns, as in (7)-(8):

    (7) *The computer with the programs of the experiment are

    broken

    (8) *The computer with the program of the experiments are

    broken.

    NO LINEAR PROXIMITY

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    18/38

    Form biasses Vigliocco & Nicol (1998):

    (9) *Are the helicopter for the flights safe?

    (10) *The helicopter for the flights are safe.

    suggest that attraction is computed on a hierarchicalstructure rather than on the final surface order. That is,they suggest that it occurs in a grammatical encodingphase before words are linearised (Franck et al. 2002;

    Franck et al. 2006). This basically accords with Bock &Millers (1991) findings (the encapsulated, formal,inflectional account).

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    19/38

    So,

    1. the overall distributivity of the phrase (semantics)

    counts,

    2. but the collectivity of local nouns (semantics) does not

    (as collectives do not attract).

    3. inflectional morphology counts (soldiers vs army),

    4. but so does supra-phrasal attraction (and even supra-

    clausal, as attraction also affects NP-pronoun co-indexings;Bock et al. 1999).

    As can be seen, experimental data soon showed what

    linguistic theory had long found out on its own: namely,that agreement is particularly sensitive to both semantic

    and formal regulation simultaneously.

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    20/38

    REMEMBER THE GRAMMAR

    THE STAFF WANT A HIGHER SALARY

    A LOT OF PEOPLE ARE UNHAPPY

    TWENTY DOLLARS IS TO MUCH

    EGGS AND BACON IS MAY FAVOURITE MEAL

    NURSELY WE: we seem a bit displeased with

    ourselftoday; see Joseph, 1979; also Harley

    and Ritter, 2002:507),

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    21/38

    Los chicos (3rd person) somos (1st person)unos idiotas

    But:

    More than one person comes/*come usually

    Votre Majest (fem) partira quand elle (fem)

    voudra Your majesty will leave whenever she wants

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    22/38

    YOU CAN

    T BLAME

    ON THE BRITS!

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    23/38

    Two current views Marking & Morphing (Bock et al. 2001;

    Eberhard et al. 2005; Bock et al. 2006). Reconciliation aligns morphological number

    and phrase number.

    Then control: copying on the verb

    *the label on the bottles ARE

    *the baby on the blankets ARE

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    24/38

    M&M 1. collectives dont attract.

    2. summation plurals (scissors, binolulars) do.

    3. Invariant plurals attract less: scissors

    (notionally singlular-like) and suds (notionallyplural-like)

    Markedness: activation boost.

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    25/38

    Maximal Input Vigliocco & Franck 2001; Vigliocco &

    Hartsuiker 2002. a level of processing is not completely isolated from

    interference from neighbouring levels, so grammatical

    encoding (the second level) may be affected by theprevious conceptualisation stage and even by the

    subsequent phonological encoding stage if circumstances

    make that advantageous for processing (models differ in

    how much interference they allow).

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    26/38

    Maximal Input Arbitrary and semantic gender; Vigliocco and

    Franck (1999)

    agreement of gender between the subject andthe predicate

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    27/38

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    28/38

    Conflicting evidence Hupet et al. (1998) and Thornton &

    MacDonald (2003) have found semanticeffects by manipulating the plausibility of the

    verb relative to the two nouns in complex NP.

    the album by the classical composers . . . BE

    praised

    the album by the classical composers . . . BEplayed

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    29/38

    Conflicting evidence 1. Local noun can exercise semantic influence

    2. effects are obtained at the verb

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    30/38

    ATTRACTION IN COMPREHENSION attraction in grammatical sentences

    Pearlmutter at al. (1999); Thornton &MacDonald (2003)

    Longer RTs at the verb

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    31/38

    Acua-Faria et al. (submitted).

    Number and gender 1.disruption, as in production, in cases of mismatch;

    2. no asymmetry;

    3. the fastest possible reaction times for number (even forfirst pass and first fixation duration), in contradistinction tothe data from English, where only regression measures

    yielded differential results (Pearlmutter et al. 1999); 4. Also surprisingly fast RTs for gender registered at the verb,

    that is, at a location prior to the actual co-indexation site;

    5. no semantic, distributivity effects, using the same materials

    with which Vigliocco et al. (1996) obtained such effects inproduction for the same language.

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    32/38

    No markedness Morphological richness

    Form precedes meaning in comp.

    Spanish has very rich dets

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    33/38

    Co-indexation speed Acua-faria (2009): E.R.P. Research, epicenes,

    perception predicates in rich inflection languages

    form clues are privileged by parsers which may dealwith them at a rate of some ten or so per second.

    M&M: since gender tends to the arbitrary in Spanish, it

    is difficult to see how gender effects may arise in asystem (designed with semantically grounded English-style number in mind) where morphing interacts with a

    previous marking stage based on the conceptualfeatures of the message. Can the feminine in mesa(table) be grounded in marking/meaning? I

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    34/38

    Co-indexation speed only blind copying operations of the kind that

    have recently been discarded seem to offer anyhope of accommodating fast (indeed pro-active)gender co-indexation bindings in the Romancelanguages in comprehension (Hawkins (1994,2004). Notice that this takes us back again to twofactors that interact opportunistically:morphology (the Romance languages) and the

    direction of encoding (comprehension vsproduction).

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    35/38

    No distributivity Vigliocco et al. (1996) did find robust

    plausibility effects. So? Berg (1998): 1. the richer the morphology the

    stronger the encapsulation of agreement

    operations from the interference of

    conceptual properties of the message; 2. what

    comes first: form or meaning.

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    36/38

    No distributiviy Lorimor, Bock, Zalkind, Sheyman & Beard

    (2008) on Russian Foote & Bock (forthcoming) on Mexican and

    Dominican.

    (For instance, syllable-final s is now reduced or weakened

    (or simply elided), and this can eliminate distinctions between

    the second and third person singular forms in almost all

    tenses and moods. Furthermore, syllable-final n can also beweakened or elided, making it difficult to distinguish between

    third person singular and plural forms (see Lunn 2002). The

    morphology of number on determiners and adjectives is also

    being lost. Similar to Andalusian Spanish)

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    37/38

    Sum In sum, the varying cross-linguistic levels of semantic affectedness/encapsulation cannot be

    predicted by any theory (linguistic or psycholinguistic) which does not incorporate the role and sizeof morphology and the direction of encoding as essential parameters. The filtering of meaningeffects by an exuberant morphology (measured, for instance, as the magnitude of distributivity

    effects) is congruent with the fact that morphological transparency boosts the signal and sopromotes accuracy. It also automatises the creation of phrasal packages even at the risk of gardenpaths.

    Since agreement is not the only form of clause-building, languages which have less of it mustcompensate for their attrition by making use of more direct conceptual influence.

    Since having a rich or a poor morphology is a matter of degree, the perennial tug-of-war betweensemantics and encapsulation in agreement with its typical cross-linguistic specificities- is naturallyaccounted for. Both processing and the grammatizalisation of processing routines must necessarilybe conveniently opportunistic. This logic applies both cross-linguistically and intra-linguistically (asit also depends on the direction of encoding).

    A clear general prediction is that notional effects should be stronger in English-style languages thanin Spanish-style ones. Another is that they should be less strong in comprehension than inproduction even in the same language.

  • 8/8/2019 Attraction Ppt

    38/38

    YOUR TURN,PLEASE.