166
i

Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

i

Page 2: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

ii

Page 3: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

iii

Anna Felicity Hobbiss

Research work coordinated by:

Oeiras, 12th July, 2016

Structural scaling and threshold

modulation of dendritic spines driven

by homeostatic plasticity

Dissertation presented to obtain the Ph.D degree in Biology | Neuroscience

Instituto de Tecnologia Química e Biológica António Xavier | Universidade Nova de Lisboa

Page 4: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

iv

Page 5: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

v

The work presented in this dissertation was carried out through the International

Neuroscience Doctoral Programme (INDP) at the Champalimaud Neuroscience

Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, under the supervision of Dr

Inbal Israely, and the thesis committee supervision of Dr Megan Carey and Dr

Marta Moita. Financial support was given by a doctoral fellowship from Fundação

para a Ciênica e Tecnologia (SFRH / BD / 51265 / 2010) and Champalimaud

Foundation.

Page 6: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

vi

Acknowledgements

As with true love, the course of a PhD never did run smooth. The journey along

my particular labyrinthine path was made possible only by being joined on it by

many people who at one time or another stepped onto the trail to walk with me.

Some were there just for a few steps; some trod the whole way next to me. Some

appeared with a map (or maybe new GPS co-ordinates) when every vestige of a

route seemed to have been lost. Others dropped by to keep me company when I

paused to rest. To all, your support, laughter, ideas, inspiration and kindness was

what kept me moving when my legs and mind were weary. Or, as a much better

writer than I put it, to “Force your heart and nerve and sinew, To serve your turn

long after they are gone, And so hold on when there is nothing in you, Except the

Will which says to them: ‘Hold on!’”.

Thank you to the Neuronal Structure and Function lab: Inbal Israely, Yazmín

Cortez, Ali Özgür Argunşah, Inês Vaz, Maria Royo, Ana Vaz, Catia Feliciano and

Daniela Pereira. I feel very lucky to have worked in a group of not just colleagues

but close friends. Through travails with the two-photon microscope to heated

biological and statistical discussions to over-eating at every lab dinner and a

hundred small moments shared in the open lab, it’s been an immense pleasure to

be part of this group. Although we had more than our share of technical problems

and experimental frustrations, the willingness of everyone to give up their time,

expertise, samples, experimental days and chocolate biscuits to help each other

as a team was truly inspiring. Inbal, thank you for your guidance, your concern

for us, your knowledge and advice on the intimidatingly immense world of

synaptic plasticity, and always making sure we had the best birthday cakes of

anyone in the CCU. Yazmínita Cortex, for taking us meninos horrorosos under

your wing and helping us to become better scientists and better people.

Özgürzinho, for being the one always travelling on the path with me, imparting

your Matlab expertise and keeping my spirit up with Turkish humour or beer.

Page 7: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

vii

Inês, for keeping me company in the microscope room countless times, giving me

Portuguese help whenever I needed it and sharing our mutual enthusiasms for

cellular biology, home-made sangria, piñatas and mole. Maria, for being a voice of

experience and wise advice about science and the PhD, and helping to keep me

calm in the face of adversity. Ana, Catia and Daniela, for all the help and support,

and making our group such a nice place to work. To my thesis committee, Megan

Carey and Marta Moita, for not only being caring and wise advisors, but also

transcending that role to become friends.

One of the pleasures of these last 7 years has been spending them with such a

warm, diverse and crazy community. The CNP is a unique and special group,

which it was my good fortune to be part of from early in its ontogeny to its

current mature form. Thanks in particular to the Ar team for helping to create

something bigger and more successful than we ever imagined at the beginning

and which I take immense pride in; to INDP 09 (definitely the best year group);

and to all those who’ve shared conversations (scientific or otherwise), beers,

costumes, thanksgiving dinners, jam sessions and much more. There isn’t enough

space to name-drop everyone that’s been significant to me but I’ll make to thank

you in person. One special shout-out: Libbi, everything was at least twice as fun

because of you as it would have been otherwise.

Lastly, to my family – my parents who amongst everything else diligently proof-

read this thesis, my siblings, my grandparents, and other family and friends in

England and around the world, for the unwavering love, support and confidence;

for reminding me there was a world outside my PhD; and for putting up with any

long monologues I may have delivered to you on synaptic plasticity and cognition

whenever you made the mistake of mentioning memory or the brain.

Page 8: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

viii

Page 9: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

ix

Table of contents

1 General introduction .......................................................................... 1 1.1 Learning and plasticity .................................................................................................. 2

1.2 Synaptic plasticity phenomena .................................................................................. 3

1.3 Homeostatic plasticity theory ..................................................................................... 6

1.4 Homeostasis at the synapse: from theory to experiments .......................... 12

1.5 Mechanisms of Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity.............................................. 16

1.6 Structural changes accompanying plasticity ..................................................... 26

1.7 Interplay between Hebbian and Homeostatic plasticity .............................. 29

1.8 Outline of work in the thesis .................................................................................... 31

1.9 Bibliography .................................................................................................................... 33

2 Structural correlates of Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity .... 47 2.1 Abstract ............................................................................................................................. 49

2.2 Introduction .................................................................................................................... 50

2.3 Materials and Methods ............................................................................................... 53

2.4 Results ................................................................................................................................ 59

2.5 Discussion ........................................................................................................................ 79

2.6 Bibliography .................................................................................................................... 84

3 Threshold modulation of LTP induction by Homeostatic

Synaptic Plasticity ..................................................................................... 89 3.1 Abstract ............................................................................................................................. 91

3.2 Introduction .................................................................................................................... 92

3.3 Materials and Methods ............................................................................................... 95

3.4 Results ................................................................................................................................ 97

3.5 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 118

3.6 Supplementary figures.............................................................................................. 124

3.7 Bibliography .................................................................................................................. 126

4 General Discussion ......................................................................... 129 4.1 Abstract ................................................................................................................... 130

4.2 Overview of work in this thesis............................................................................. 131

4.3 Beyond synaptic scaling – silent synapses and network function ......... 132

4.4 Heterosynaptic plasticity and synaptic clustering ........................................ 134

4.5 Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity in health and disease ............................... 137

4.6 The role of sleep in homeostasis .......................................................................... 140

4.7 Conclusion ...................................................................................................................... 141

4.8 Bibliography .................................................................................................................. 143

Page 10: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

x

Table of figures

Figure 1.1 Homeostatic feedback regulation in the nervous system ............................. 5

Figure 1.2 Synaptic scaling following chronic activity perturbations ........................... 8

Figure 2.1 Hippocampal organotypic slice cultures as a study system for

Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity ........................................................................................ 54

Figure 2.2 Acute activity blockade does not cause structural changes ...................... 60

Figure 2.3 Activity block induces functional Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity ....... 62

Figure 2.4 Structural correlates of Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity ........................... 64

Figure 2.5 Distributions of spine morphologies are not affected by HSP at 48

hours ............................................................................................................................................. 66

Figure 2.6 Dendrite thickness and spine density are not affected by activity

blockade ...................................................................................................................................... 68

Figure 2.7 Firing rates return to control levels after removal of activity block ..... 70

Figure 2.8 Removal of activity block reverses HSP-dependent spine volume

increase ........................................................................................................................................ 73

Figure 2.9 Loss of Tsc1 does not impair structural HSP ................................................... 76

Figure 2.10 Chronic GABAA blockade does not induce downscaling through HSP

......................................................................................................................................................... 78

Figure 3.1 Protocol for uncaging experiments...................................................................... 98

Figure 3.2 LTP magnitude and longevity following HSP ................................................ 100

Figure 3.3 Spine growth dynamics during stimulation ................................................... 102

Figure 3.4 LTP induction had increased efficacy following HSP ................................. 105

Figure 3.5 Small spines show greater LTP after HSP ....................................................... 106

Figure 3.6 Behaviour of different sized spines during LTP ........................................... 109

Figure 3.7 Neighbouring spines respond to stimulation after HSP ........................... 112

Figure 3.8 Reduced threshold for LTP after HSP ............................................................... 115

Figure 3.9 Capacity for LTP is maintained after 72 hours of activity block ........... 117

Figure 3.10 Post-stimulation growth for real stimulations and sham stimulations

....................................................................................................................................................... 124

Figure 3.11 LTP magnitude grouped by spine volume ................................................... 124

Figure 3.12 Neighbouring spine parameters ....................................................................... 125

Figure 4.1 Structural clustering after cooperation and competition ........................ 135

Page 11: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

xi

Glossary of abbreviations and chemicals

Name/ Acronym

Complete name Function

APV 2R-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid NMDA receptor antagonist

ASD Autism Spectrum Disorder

Bicuculline GABAA antagonist

CNQX AMPA receptor antagonist

FMRP Fragile X Mental Retardation Protein

GluA1 Glutamate AMPA receptor 1 Receptor subunit of the AMPA receptor

GluA2 Glutamate AMPA receptor 2 Receptor subunit of the AMPA receptor

HSP Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity

LTD Long Term Depression Synaptic weakening following activity

LTP Long Term Potentiation Synaptic strengthening following activity

mEPSC mini Excitatory Post-Synaptic Current Excitatory current caused by spontaneous pre-synaptic glutamate release

mTOR Mammalian Target of Rapamycin Signalling molecule fundamental in the mTOR signalling pathway

NO Nitric oxide Retrograde signal important for plasticity

PTP Post Tetanic Potential The large increase in potentiation immediately following a stimulation

PTX Picrotoxin GABAA antagonist

RA all-trans Retinoic Acid

TEA Tetraethylammonium chloride K+ channel blocker

TNFα Tumour Necrosis Factor α

TSC1 Tuberous Sclerosis Complex 1 A repressor of the mTOR pathway

TTX Tetrodotoxin Sodium channel blocker

Page 12: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

xii

Page 13: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

xiii

“Now, here, you see, it takes all the running you can do, to keep in the same

place. If you want to get somewhere else, you must run at least twice as fast as

that!”

The Red Queen

‘Through the Looking-Glass and What Alice Found There’ by Lewis Carroll

Page 14: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

xiv

Resumo

Os circuitos neuronais empregam mecanismos de plasticidade sináptica

homeostática (em inglês homeostatic synaptic plasticity, HSP) para manter a

atividade neuronal dentro de um intervalo óptimo, contrariando a tendência de

desvios na plasticidade do tipo Hebbiano, tanto LTP (long-term potentiation)

como LTD (long-term depression). Este mecanismo manifesta-se por uma

adaptação dos botões sinápticos, tanto aumentando como diminuindo a atividade

neuronal geral. Nesta tese de doutoramento investigamos as estruturas que se

correlacionam com a indução de HSP e determinamos como é que esta forma de

modulação sináptica tem um impacto na capacidade de troca de informação

durante a plasticidade Hebbiana. Neste estudo demonstramos que o bloqueio

prolongado da atividade de culturas organotípicas de hipocampo causa o

crescimento das espinhas dendríticas, complementando o incremento fisiológico

da capacidade sináptica. Este crescimento é reversível, voltando aos níveis

normais uma vez removido o bloqueio. Utilizando microscopia de dois fotões

e uncaging de glutamato, investigamos a fundo como é que sinapses que

sofreram adaptação provocada por HSP respondem à plasticidade Hebbiana.

Neste estudo descobrimos que 48 horas após o bloqueio da atividade neuronal o

LTP é mantido, demonstrando que a capacidade de transmissão de informação

no circuito é mantida mesmo após a adaptação. Além disso, verificamos que há

um aumento da longevidade da potenciação de espinhas dendríticas individuais,

quando comparado com neurónios nos quais a atividade nunca foi restringida, e

que o threshold para a indução de plasticidade é menor. Curiosamente,

descobrimos que a expressão de LTP é diferencialmente modulada dependendo

do tamanho inicial das espinhas dendríticas. Em espinhas pequenas a indução de

plasticidade é mais eficaz após HSP, enquanto as espinhas de grandes dimensões

são menos influenciadas por este tipo de plasticidade. Estes achados revelam que

o mecanismo de modulação induzido durante HSP resulta numa alteração da

Page 15: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

xv

distribuição do tamanho das espinhas dendríticas, ocorrendo esta modificação

preferencialmente em espinhas de menores dimensões. Estes resultados

demonstram que a capacidade do circuito de codificar informação é mantida

após a HSP. No entanto, após manipulação da atividade neuronal global, espinhas

individuais podem ser moduladas, permitindo que a rede neuronal regule a troca

de informação dentro de uma amplitude ótima.

Page 16: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

xvi

Abstract

Neural networks employ homeostatic synaptic plasticity (HSP) to maintain

activity within an optimal range, countering the tendencies of unchecked

Hebbian LTP or LTD to saturate the network. It is manifested by synaptic scaling

of all the inputs on a neuron, either upwards to increase global activity or

downwards to decrease it. In this thesis we investigate the structural correlates

that accompany the induction of homeostatic plasticity and then determine how

this form of synaptic modulation impacts the ability of inputs to undergo

Hebbian plasticity. We show that prolonged activity blockade in organotypic

hippocampal slices causes structural growth of individual dendritic spines,

complementing the physiological increase in synaptic strength. This volume

increase is reversible, returning to control levels after the activity block is lifted.

Using glutamate uncaging and 2-photon imaging, we further investigate how

synapses which have undergone synaptic scaling respond to Hebbian plasticity.

We find that after 48 hrs of activity blockade, LTP is maintained, showing that the

ability for the network to encode information is retained. Furthermore the

longevity of potentiation of single spines is increased when compared to neurons

in which activity has not been restricted, and the threshold for plasticity

induction is decreased. Interestingly, we find that the expression of LTP is

differentially modulated depending on the initial size of the spines. In small

spines, the induction of plasticity is more efficacious after HSP, whereas larger

spines are less impacted by this form of plasticity. After HSP neighbouring spines

express short term structural plasticity. Our findings demonstrate that the gain

modulation that is induced during HSP results in a shift in the size distribution of

dendritic spines, but that such tuning is expressed preferentially at smaller

inputs. These results illustrate that after HSP the information-encoding capacity

of a network is conserved, but that synapses are individually tuned, allowing the

network to regulate the strength of its inputs to function within an optimal range.

Page 17: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

xvii

Page 18: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

xviii

Page 19: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

1

Chapter 1

1 General introduction

General Introduction: Homeostatic Synaptic

Plasticity and Nervous system

functioning

Page 20: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

2

1.1 Learning and plasticity

Living organisms are defined by two fundamental traits: their genetic make-up,

modulated by their environmental experience. One of the essential features of

organisms is their ability to sense their environment and respond accordingly,

whether it be by locomoting to a chemical stimulus in the case of bacteria,

changing patterns of cellular growth to orient towards a light source in flowering

plants, or altering gene expression patterns to metabolise a new substrate in

yeast. In the animal clade the evolution of the nervous system has made its

members uniquely accomplished at responding to stimuli presented by the

external world. Animal neurophysiology and the resultant behaviour are effected

by a combination of genetic and experience-dependent components, which allow

an animal to adapt its behaviour rapidly to better fit the environment. These

behavioural changes, known as learning, occur when some aspects of the

nervous system are modified, leading it to produce a different output – the

learned output – to the same input pattern of activation. The strength of learning

is correlated to the valence of the experience. Strongly affective stimuli, either

positive such as food consumption, or negative such as pain, will lead to bigger

consequent changes in the nervous system. These in turn will make the

behaviour which causes the experience more likely or less likely respectively.

Nervous systems are extremely well suited to quick and profound changes

because their complex multi-layered organisation allows easy modification of

their topography and functioning. This permits animals the fast environmental

adaption that has made them such a supremely diverse and successful phylum.

The ability of nervous systems to change their firing pattern and topography as a

response to activity is called neural plasticity. This broad term encompasses a

very large range of different mechanisms which can alter the activity of a neural

network. The mechanisms vary in timescales (from milliseconds to the lifetime

of an animal), in the process of induction and in the mode of implementation in

Page 21: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

3

the biological system. This thesis will focus on one of these types of plasticity: the

change of strength of synapses (the connections between neurons) known as

synaptic plasticity. This mechanism has received a great deal of theoretical and

experimental attention since it is considered one of the main biological

substrates for memory storage, due to its ability to change circuit functioning

and endure throughout an animal’s lifetime. This intense field of study has

yielded a huge literature on types of synaptic plasticity and modes of induction,

expression and maintenance. In the remainder of the thesis I will discuss two

forms of synaptic plasticity which perform fundamentally different roles within

the nervous system: Hebbian plasticity and Homeostatic plasticity.

1.2 Synaptic plasticity phenomena

1.2.1 Hebbian plasticity

In 1949, Donald Hebb proposed that if synapses could respond to a particular

input pattern by becoming stronger, this could instantiate learning within the

network (Hebb, 1949). Alterations of behaviour paralleling synaptic changes

were demonstrated in the mid 1960s in Aplysia (Kandel and Tauc, 1965). In 1973

the first experimental evidence was discovered of synaptic strengthening caused

by strong activity, and was termed Long Term Potentiation or LTP (Bliss and

Lømo, 1973). A few years later, the discovery of Long Term Depression (LTD) as

a counterpart to LTP showed that induced changes can be bi-directional (Lynch

et al., 1977). LTP and LTD came to be known collectively as Hebbian plasticity, in

accordance with Hebb’s postulate. By changing the connection strengths in a

network, Hebbian plasticity allows it to encode information, and thus it is

regarded as a major mechanism for learning and memory.

For Hebbian plasticity to be an efficient and robust mechanism for memory

Page 22: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

4

storage, it requires two important features. Firstly, it must be input specific –

that is, the changes to synaptic strength must occur at the same place that the

input signal was received, to ensure faithful encoding of the stimulus. Secondly, it

must be long-lasting, to be a realistic substrate for memories which can last for a

lifetime. These two aims are achieved through precisely calibrated signalling

mechanisms, ultimately leading to stable molecular and structural changes at the

level of the synapse, dendritic branch and whole cell.

1.2.2 Homeostatic plasticity

Although it is an extremely effective memory storage system, the

implementation of Hebbian plasticity also entails a problem, in that it is

intrinsically unstable. An increase in synaptic strengths, such as in LTP, would

increase the excitatory drive on to a post-synaptic cell, making it more likely to

fire. This in turn would increase the likelihood of more LTP; thus positive

feedback would quickly saturate the system, resulting in a hyperactive state with

saturated synaptic inputs. Conversely, excessive LTD would also proliferate,

resulting in a silent state with inputs fully depressed. Modelling studies of

Hebbian learning revealed this problem, leading to the need to introduce a

stabilising mechanism to keep the activity of the network within a dynamic

range (Malsburg, 1973; Miller and MacKay, 1994). Mechanisms which enact this

type of compensatory changes would come to be called Homeostatic Plasticity, in

accordance with other well established homeostatic mechanisms to control

physiological parameters such as body temperature and blood glucose (Cannon,

1932). All of these mechanisms use a negative feedback system, such that if the

parameter exceeds the limits of the optimal range, compensatory pathways are

activated to push it back to within the correct boundaries. In the specific case of

nervous system activity, two distinct but related parameters need to be kept

within functioning ranges (Figure 1.1). One is network firing rates, which if left

unchecked could reach pathologically high (epileptic) or low (silent) levels. The

Page 23: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

5

other is synaptic strengths, because it is a variable range of synaptic weights

which gives a network such an immense capacity for information storage. If all

synapses become saturated or conversely fully depressed, the information

stored in the cell and network will be lost. Conveniently, the regulation of

synaptic strengths will also have the benefit of maintaining the network activity

levels in check, so both of these parameters can be adjusted by controlling the

range of synaptic weights of a neuron.

Figure 1.1 Homeostatic feedback regulation in the nervous system

Activity in the nervous system has an optimal level of activity, whether measured

through action potential firing or synaptic strength. If, through changes in input or

Hebbian plasticity processes, the level of activity is pushed out of the ideal bounds, the

processing of the network and its storage capacity will be compromised. Homeostatic

processes use negative feedback mechanisms to restore the system parameters to their

ideal intermediate state.

Page 24: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

6

1.3 Homeostatic plasticity theory

The first studies to show that loss of function could induce compensatory

changes in the nervous system were at the neuromuscular junction. Since the

middle of the 20th century, muscles deprived of innervation from motor neurons

were known to show increased sensitivity to neurotransmitters such as

acetylcholine and to applied current, a phenomenon known as ‘denervation

sensitivity’ (Axelsson and Thesleff, 1959). This was described in multiple

systems, from the frog skeletal muscle to cat gastrocnemius and iris muscles

(Brown, 1937; Nicholls, 1956; Axelsson and Thesleff, 1959). In central

mammalian neurons, bi-directional shifts in firing rates of cultured cortical

neurons were demonstrated in the early 1990s (Ramakers et al., 1990; Corner

and Ramakers, 1991). A long-term blockade of activity caused hyper-excitability

once the blockade was released, whilst chronically increased activity lead to

decreased firing rates. No mechanism that could account for these changes was

proposed however, and the findings were not linked to homeostasis of the

network. Instead they were attributed to an altered time course of development.

Meanwhile, studies in invertebrate central pattern generators revealed that if

rhythmic activity was disturbed by an experimental perturbation, over time

compensatory changes could restore the rhythmicity of the circuit (Turrigiano et

al., 1994; Thoby-Brisson and Simmers, 1998). In 1994, Miller and Mackay

theorised that network stabilisation could be achieved at a synaptic level, a

mechanism which I will henceforth refer to as Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity

(HSP) (Miller and MacKay, 1994). The detailed mechanism they proposed was a

“non-specific decay of all synaptic strengths, provided the rate of this decay is set

for the cell as a whole to cancel the total increase due to specific, Hebbian

plasticity”. In essence, rather than any one particular synapse or set of synapses

changing weight to compensate for activity changes, the whole synaptic

population is affected equally, such that whilst the overall sum synaptic

strengths and therefore activity of a neuron is changed, the relative weight

Page 25: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

7

differences between synapses is preserved. The advantage of such a system, as

opposed to other proposed methods such as a ‘competition rule’ between inputs

(Malsburg, 1973), is that differences in weights between synapses are still

preserved, so the computational and storage capacity of the network is not

compromised and HSP will not erase the information set by Hebbian plasticity.

1.3.1 Synaptic scaling

The process outlined by Miller and Mackay can be mathematically expressed as a

multiplicative scaling of synaptic strengths (Figure 1.2). The first experimental

evidence for such synaptic scaling came from experiments in primary cultures of

rat cortical neurons (Turrigiano et al., 1998). Activity was chronically perturbed

by application of pharmacological agents for 48 hours, using either Bicuculline (a

GABAA receptor antagonist) to increase firing rates by preventing inhibitory

transmission, or Tetrodotoxin (TTX) (a voltage-gated Na+ channel antagonist) to

block all cell firing. Synaptic strengths were assayed by measuring miniature

excitatory post-synaptic currents (mEPSCs) of treated cells. These occur through

spontaneous (i.e. action potential-independent) pre-synaptic neurotransmitter

release. The size of the post-synaptic current (as measured by whole-cell patch

clamp at the soma) is proportional to the strength of the synapse; thus a

recording of mEPSCs can be used to sample the synaptic strengths of the

recorded neuron. Bicuculline application for 48 hrs led to initially increased

activity, and compensatorily decreased mEPSC amplitudes. Conversely, TTX

application causing activity blockade led to increased mEPSC amplitudes. In

keeping with the mechanism proposed by Miller and MacKay, perturbations of

activity caused a linear multiplicative scaling of mEPSC amplitudes, implying that

all the synapses had been altered proportionally by the same scaling factor. This

and subsequent work supporting work established HSP as a novel form of neural

plasticity by which the output of a system can be modulated whilst the

Page 26: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

8

information content is preserved (O’Brien et al., 1998; Turrigiano et al., 1998;

Desai et al., 1999). Since it requires the integration of network activity,

homeostatic plasticity acts at long timescales (on the order of hours to days), and

in a global, cell-to-network-wide fashion, as opposed to the fast, single synapse

resolution of Hebbian plasticity.

1.3.2 Other types of Homeostatic Plasticity

Since the initial discovery, it has become clear that homeostatic plasticity is a

broad class of phenomena acting at different levels and through different

mechanisms, a combination of which may be co-opted for use in any one

situation (Davis, 2006, 2013; Karmarkar and Buonomano, 2006; Maffei and

Turrigiano, 2008; O’Leary and Wyllie, 2011; Turrigiano, 2011). As well as the

aforementioned post-synaptic scaling, network stabilisation can be achieved in

Figure 1.2 Synaptic scaling following chronic activity perturbations

Different sizes synapses have a different AMPA receptor content which is set by Hebbian

plasticity processes. Chronic changes in activity cause the population of synapses to scale the

number of AMPA receptors in the post-synaptic density either down or up, to counteract the

shift. The relative numbers of AMPA receptors, and thus the strengths of the synapses, is

preserved after the scaling has taken place.

Page 27: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

9

numerous other ways (mirroring the abundant mechanisms for instantiating

Hebbian plasticity (Nelson and Turrigiano, 2008)). The most prominent of these

alternative means of establishing homeostasis are briefly described below.

1.3.2.1 Pre-synaptic homeostatic plasticity

Pre-synaptic changes in LTP and LTD are well established, and have more

recently been confirmed as a mechanism of homeostatic plasticity, in systems as

diverse as the neuromuscular junction in Drosophila and Xenopus to central

nervous system synapses (Davis and Müller, 2014). One interesting question is

whether the pre-synaptic changes are triggered by aberrant levels of activity in a

cell autonomous way, or whether trans-synaptic signalling from the post-

synaptic partner is the requisite signal of activity levels. In fact it is likely that

both mechanisms are at play (Jakawich et al., 2010), although there is increasing

evidence to suggest that a retrograde signal from the post to the pre-synaptic

side is the major determinant controlling aspects of pre-synaptic strength

(Petersen et al., 1997; Davis et al., 1998; Nick and Ribera, 2000; Frank et al.,

2006; Branco et al., 2008; Lindskog et al., 2010). The identity of the signal is yet

to be discovered, although in Drosophila TOR signalling is crucial (Penney et al.,

2012) and endostatin has been identified as a possible candidate (Wang et al.,

2014). Various different modifications of pre-synaptic function have been

observed, including quantal size (i.e. how much neurotransmitter each pre-

synaptic vesicle holds) (Davis et al., 1998), speed of vesicle uptake (Thiagarajan

et al., 2005), number of docked vesicles (Murthy et al., 2001) and release

probability (Sandrock et al., 1997; Davis et al., 1998; Frank et al., 2006; Lindskog

et al., 2010; Zhao et al., 2011; Mitra et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2016). The latter

mechanism may be caused by changes in calcium influx into the pre-synaptic

bouton which promotes vesicle fusion (Frank et al., 2009; Zhao et al., 2011),

mediated by plasticity in calcium channel expression levels in pre-synaptic

terminals (Jensen et al., 2009).

Page 28: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

10

1.3.2.2 Intrinsic threshold of excitability

An alternative to changing the strength of synaptic signalling is to shift the

intrinsic excitability of the cell, so that for the same amount of synaptic input the

output activity is either increased or decreased (Zhang and Linden, 2003;

Turrigiano, 2011). This can be achieved by altering the expression of ion

channels in the cell membrane, thus changing the threshold at which a cell will

fire. This type of plasticity was observed in early studies on the lobster

somatogastric ganglion circuit, where neurons isolated from their normal inputs

regain their in vivo bursting patterns after 3 – 4 days in a calcium-dependent

manner (Turrigiano et al., 1994). Changes in intrinsic excitability have been

observed both in vitro following activity blockade and in vivo after sensory

deprivation (Desai et al., 1999; Breton and Stuart, 2009). Various channels have

been implicated in changing the membrane conductance. Very clearly important

are potassium channels, which are down-regulated in response to chronic

activity decrease (Desai et al., 1999; Cudmore et al., 2010; Lee and Chung, 2014;

Lee et al., 2015). In addition, the HCN channel, which reduces excitability by

increasing the leak current of the membrane, is down-regulated in dendrites in

response to sensory deprivation (Breton and Stuart, 2009). Meanwhile, sodium

channel conductance is up-regulated after activity blockade (Desai et al., 1999).

These changes to intrinsic excitability appear to require calcium signalling

(especially for down-shifts after excessive activity), with both L-type calcium

channels and NMDA channels being implicated (O’Leary et al., 2010; Lee and

Chung, 2014). Various generalised models have shown that if multiple different

channels can adjust their levels based on activity, the circuit activity is robust to

perturbations (Liu et al., 1998; O’Leary et al., 2014). One interesting thing to note

about plasticity of intrinsic excitability is that, although it may take hours to days

to develop (in keeping with other types of homeostatic plasticity), the actual

switch between different patterns of firing can take less than 1 hr (Turrigiano et

al., 1994). This occurs because small changes to conductances can flip neurons

Page 29: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

11

between two bistable activity states, one with low levels of firing and one with

bursting behaviour. A system can therefore potentially change between firing

behaviours very quickly by employing this type of plasticity, allowing for fast

adaption to different levels of activity.

1.3.2.3 Excitation-inhibition balance

Network activity can be altered by changing the balance between excitation and

inhibition (E/I ratio) in the neural circuit. The E/I ratio has been especially

studied in the cortex, where it affects many functions such as gain and feature

selectivity (Barth et al., 2004). Its role in homeostatic plasticity was confirmed in

vivo after rats were dark reared for 2 days, resulting in an increased excitatory

and decreased inhibitory drive (Maffei et al., 2004). Successive studies have also

demonstrated how changes to the E/I ratio can balance network activity whilst

still maintaining cortical function (Atallah and Scanziani, 2009; Pouille et al.,

2009; Xue et al., 2014). For example, a neuron receiving inputs with an increased

strength has an increased threshold to fire due to correspondingly enhanced

feed-forward inhibition (Pouille et al., 2009). This has the effect of stabilising the

system, whilst simultaneously making the cortex sensitive to a wide range of

input strengths. In the hippocampus, balanced levels of excitation and inhibition

produced gamma oscillations which are critical for its function (Atallah and

Scanziani, 2009). Indeed, individual cells themselves receive balanced amounts

of excitation and inhibition, stabilising output in a cell-autonomous manner (Xue

et al., 2014). One candidate for modulating the relative strengths of excitation

and inhibition is nitric oxide (NO), which is known to play fundamental roles in

Hebbian pre-synaptic plasticity, and also in homeostasis for both excitatory and

inhibitory cells (reviewed in (Hardingham et al., 2013)); as well as other

signalling molecules such as Npas4 (Spiegel et al., 2014).

In addition to changes to the E/I balance, homeostatic changes to inhibitory

activity alone have become the focus of increasing interest (Wenner, 2011).

Page 30: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

12

Classic studies on visual deprivation documented that the density of GABAergic

cells was reduced following the loss of visual input or activity deprivation

(Hendry and Jones, 1986; Benevento et al., 1995; Rutherford et al., 1997, 1998).

Detailed studies into mechanisms have revealed that inhibitory synapses show

many of the same homeostatic mechanisms when exposed to activity

perturbations as excitatory synapses, including bidirectional shifts in activity

(Karmarkar and Buonomano, 2006), synaptic scaling (Kilman et al., 2002;

Holopainen and Lauren, 2003; Chang et al., 2010) and pre-synaptic changes

(Hartman et al., 2006). This being said the relationship between activity shifts

and inhibition is not a simple one, since visual deprivation during the critical

period can also cause potentiation of inhibition rather than the expected

depression (Maffei et al., 2006; Nahmani and Turrigiano, 2014). These results

suggest that the timing of the activity shift is crucial for determining how the

system will respond.

1.4 Homeostasis at the synapse: from theory to experiments

1.4.1 Computational modelling of Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity

The homeostatic synaptic plasticity field has a rich history of combining

theoretical predictions and experimental findings. Indeed modelling studies

were quick to reveal the intrinsic instability of Hebbian processes, leading to the

first proposals of homeostatic mechanisms. Initial papers exploring the

computational dynamics of LTP and LTD had to compensate for the positive

feedback inherent in Hebbian theory, for instance by providing constraints to the

expressed plasticity (Oja, 1982), by setting an overall constant sum synaptic

strength thus forcing competition for plasticity between inputs (Malsburg,

1973), or by using a sliding threshold of plasticity to balance LTP and LTD as

seen in the seminal BCM rule of Hebbian plasticity (Bienenstock et al., 1982).

Page 31: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

13

Initially these stabilising mechanisms were built into the Hebbian models as a

fundamental aspect of the plasticity. Opposing this, a train of thought proposing

that in fact Hebbian and homeostatic plasticity were separable processes began

to arise. As was referred to earlier in this chapter, the first explored mechanism

cell-wide synaptic strength changes proposed that a multiplicative decay (as

opposed to a linear decay) would preserve information content in the network

whilst constraining the output activity (Miller and MacKay, 1994).

Since these beginnings, the computational theory of homeostatic plasticity has

expanded hugely, with the development of models which reflect realistic

physiological parameters for synaptic strength changes. HSP has been shown

influence Hebbian plasticity (Litwin-Kumar and Doiron, 2014), STDP (Clopath et

al., 2010), connectivity (Tetzlaff et al., 2011) and working memory (Renart et al.,

2003). Interestingly, some discrepancies between theory and experimental data

have arisen. In particular, the timescale over which homeostasis occurs is still

under debate. A recent study formulated a model where homeostasis needs to

have a fast rate detector (on the order of seconds to minutes) to stabilise

Hebbian plasticity (Zenke et al., 2013). Experimentally however, HSP is induced

over hours to days (see parts 1.5.1.1 and 1.5.1.2 for more details). Very recently a

fast form of pre-synaptic homeostasis was reported which could fulfil the

conditions required (Wang et al., 2016). Future work theoretical and

experimental is needed to resolve how the different timescales interact in

dynamic systems.

1.4.2 The spatial expression and scale of HSP

Homeostatic synaptic plasticity has been shown to occur widely across the

nervous system. The majority of studies have been conducted in cultured cells,

from the cortex (Turrigiano et al., 1998), hippocampus (Lissin et al., 1998),

peripheral neurons such as spinal cord (O’Brien et al., 1998), and somatogastric

ganglion cells (Turrigiano et al., 1994). In vivo homeostatic synaptic plasticity in

Page 32: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

14

various systems has also been demonstrated, although fewer studies exist due to

the increased difficulty of the experimental protocol. The majority of studies are

from sensory cortices, where activity reduction can be achieved through sensory

deprivation protocols such as ocular deprivation (Desai et al., 2002; Maffei et al.,

2004; Goel and Lee, 2007; Maffei and Turrigiano, 2008; Keck et al., 2013) or

whisker trimming (Greenhill et al., 2015). Other sub-cortical areas such as the

hippocampus (Pawlak et al., 2005; Echegoyen et al., 2007) and superior

colliculus (Chandrasekaran et al., 2007), and the spinal cord (Knogler et al.,

2010; Garcia-Bereguiain et al., 2013) have also been used to demonstrate

homeostatic synaptic responses. The details of in vivo homeostasis and its

implications for nervous system functioning are discussed later in this chapter in

Section 1.5.2.

A complicating factor for the study of homeostasis is that although the basic

processes employed seem to be universal, different synapses show different

responses to shifts in activity. Even within single structures such as the

hippocampus, different synaptic sites (dentate to CA3 and CA3 to CA1) show

differing or even opposing responses (Kim and Tsien, 2008; Lee et al., 2013).

Compounding the somewhat bewildering array of modes of synaptic

homeostasis is the question of the spatial scale on which the homeostatic signal

acts. Processes such as regulation of the excitation/inhibition balance are by

definition network-level phenomena, but synaptic processes can be the result of

much smaller-scale integrations of activity. Synaptic homeostasis is normally

described and modelled as a cell-wide process, whereby neuron activity is

integrated over long time-periods and all spines are then scaled in concert.

Theoretically, this type of cell-wide scaling differentiates HSP from Hebbian

plasticity and so the information encoded in synaptic weights is not lost

(Toyoizumi et al., 2014). Although there is a weight of experimental evidence to

support wide-scale scaling, most of the induction protocols involve a global up or

down-regulation of activity, so it is not possible to differentiate between global

Page 33: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

15

or local plasticity processes. In fact, studies looking at smaller neuronal

segments, from individual dendrites (Sutton et al., 2006; Branco et al., 2008)

down to single spines (Hou et al., 2008, 2011; Béïque et al., 2011), have indeed

seen evidence for homeostasis occurring at these levels, in both the upward and

downward directions. This spine-specific scaling was induced by manipulating

the activity levels of upstream cells, and then examining the post-synaptic sites

they connected to. Its expression involved changes in AMPAR content in the

spines, similarly to global induction protocols (Hou et al., 2008, 2011).

Neighbouring spines receiving normal levels of input remained unchanged,

suggesting that homeostatic regulation was local to the individual synapse. In the

visual cortex in vivo however, selective synapse silencing produced a

potentiation of all synapses including those not affected by the deprivation

protocol, suggesting that a more global homeostatic effect occurs (Greenhill et al.,

2015). The potential for synapse-specific homeostasis presents an interesting

conundrum for how HSP and Hebbian plasticity can co-exist, since the induction

of one type would seem to be antagonistic to the other. In Hebbian plasticity,

reduced signalling (i.e. low levels of calcium in the spine) leads to depression of

the spine, but in homeostasis reduced activity would lead to potentiation. It will

be intriguing in the future to see whether more evidence for synapse-specific

HSP emerges.

Interestingly, there is also disagreement in the literature about whether post-

synaptic homeostasis is induced by changes in pre-synaptic activity (i.e. the

synapse’s own glutamatergic drive), or post-synaptic activity (implying some

integration of the activity of the post-synaptic cell). Two studies saw a

decoupling of post-synaptic spiking activity with synaptic scaling, leading to the

conclusion that neurotransmission is the crucial induction signal for scaling (Hou

et al., 2008; Fong et al., 2015). Other studies (Burrone et al., 2002; Ibata et al.,

2008; Goold and Nicoll, 2010) have demonstrated that indeed activity changes in

only the post-synaptic cell do change the input synaptic strength. It is likely

Page 34: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

16

therefore that both pre- and post-synaptic changes have the capacity to induce

homeostasis.

1.5 Mechanisms of Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity

1.5.1 Molecular pathways of HSP

Following the discovery of HSP an intense area of research has been to uncover

the molecular pathways involved. Unsurprisingly given the diversity of

homeostatic phenomena, many different molecules have been seen to play a role

in HSP. Various overlapping pathways have been identified, which depend on the

protocol used to induce HSP. Below I will discuss some of the different molecular

signatures of these alternate HSP forms.

1.5.1.1 Up-scaling induced by reduction in excitatory drive

The original synaptic scaling discovery used TTX, a sodium channel blocker, to

prevent spiking activity in cultured neurons (Turrigiano et al., 1998). This has

the effect of both blocking evoked neurotransmission (i.e. reducing excitatory

drive) and also post-synaptic spiking. Notably however, miniature transmission

(i.e. spontaneous vesicle fusion) still occurs under these conditions, so in

contrast to some of the protocols described further on in this chapter, there is

not a total absence of AMPA and NMDA activation. This difference is important

mechanistically to determine the time-course and molecular identity of the

scaling (Sutton et al., 2006). Sensory deprivation in vivo, which again has the

result of reducing excitatory drive without pharmacologically blocking any of the

receptors, should also induce this same type of HSP.

The synaptic strength changes of all different types of HSP appear to be mediated

by changes in the AMPA receptor content at the synapse. Activity reduction

protocols result in an increased AMPAR content causing functional up-scaling,

Page 35: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

17

whilst increased activity leads to down-scaling due to less synaptic AMPAR. The

AMPAR subunit required for the expression of the scaling differs depending on

the induction protocol. There are 4 AMPAR subunit types; GluA1, GluA2 and

GluA3, and GluA4, which have different functional properties. The full AMPA

receptor is tetrameric, normally consisting of dimers of dimers which commonly

combine GluA2 with either GluA1, 3 or 4 (Isaac et al., 2007). GluA2-lacking

AMPARs have an important functional role to play in synaptic plasticity since

they are calcium permeable, so will enhance calcium-dependent signalling

pathways in synapses. GluA2-lacking AMPARs are thus known to be involved in

the early stages of LTP, whereas GluA2-containing receptors are inserted at later

stages during what seems to be a consolidation process.

When excitatory drive is reduced using either TTX or sensory deprivation, both

the GluA1 and GluA2 subunits are up-regulated at synapses in a correlated

fashion (Wierenga et al., 2005). It is GluA2 however that appears be to the crucial

component, since knock down of GluA2 but not GluA1 abolishes scaling (Gainey

et al., 2009). GluA2 is also inserted on a surprisingly fast timescale, with changes

being visible after only 4 hours of activity block (despite most protocols of

scaling showing changes only at the 24 hour timepoint) (Ibata et al., 2008).

GluA1 may also have a role to play in the functional expression of scaling

however, since phosphorylation of this subunit through PKA can cause up-

scaling (Diering et al., 2014). Similarly single-spine induction protocols also

appear to up-regulate, and require, GluA2-lacking AMPA receptors (identifiable

through pharmacological or electrophysiological methods) (Hou et al., 2008;

Béïque et al., 2011), as do some developmental homeostasis processes in vivo

(Garcia-Bereguiain et al., 2013). Further on in this chapter I will explore the

important role that GluA2-lacking receptors play in a different type of synaptic

homeostasis, a fast retinoic acid-dependent form (see section 1.5.1.2).

Although AMPA receptors appear to be the clear candidate for the functional

Page 36: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

18

expression of scaling, a lot of work remains to understand the molecular

mechanisms involved. An increasingly complex set of pathways is emerging to

span the gap between the start point of sensing of activity changes, to the

insertion or removal of the AMPARs at the other end. As with other types of

plasticity, calcium is looking increasingly promising as a candidate for initiating

the plastic changes (Liu et al., 1998). In particular, a drop in Ca2+ levels at the

soma can induce synaptic scaling (Ibata et al., 2008); a mechanism which makes

intuitive sense for the detection of cell-wide activity (as opposed to local

dendritic calcium changes which initiate Hebbian plasticity). The change in Ca2+

concentration appears to be sensed by CaMKIV, one of the calcium/calmodulin-

dependent protein kinase (CaMK) family (Ibata et al., 2008). CaMKs play many

important signalling roles in neurons, especially in plasticity through their role in

calcium sensing. Another member of the family, CaMKII, is an extremely

important and abundant signalling molecule in Hebbian synaptic plasticity. Two

of the subunits which compose the complete CaMKII molecule, CaMKIIα and

CaMKIIβ, change in proportion in response to activity blockade with TTX

(Thiagarajan et al., 2002), with β levels rising whilst α is down-regulated (this

shift is reversed after chronic increase of activity with bicuculline). β subunits

have a higher sensitivity to calcium so are activated by lower calcium levels. This

shift will therefore act to sensitise the quietened system to any activity. CaMKII

in turn regulates the recruitment of GKAP, a post-synaptic scaffolding protein

which is necessary for synaptic scaling (Shin et al., 2012).

Biology is a frugal system, and in HSP, as in multiple other physiological

processes, molecules have been co-opted from other purposes to form part of

these signalling pathways. For example, Tumour Necrosis Factor α (TNFα), a

pro-inflammatory cytokine, is crucial for the synaptic scaling process, both in

vitro (Stellwagen and Malenka, 2006) and in vivo (Kaneko et al., 2008; Steinmetz

and Turrigiano, 2010), where it acts to maintain the synapses in an up-scaled

state (Steinmetz and Turrigiano, 2010). Importantly, it is not required for LTP or

Page 37: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

19

LTD (Stellwagen and Malenka, 2006; Kaneko et al., 2008; Knogler et al., 2010) – a

crucial clue that at the molecular level these different types of plasticity can be

differentiated. Adding another component to the HSP signalling circuit, TNFα is

derived from glia surrounding the neurons undergoing scaling (Stellwagen and

Malenka, 2006), through their sensitivity to glutamate signalling. It is possible

that HSP can co-opt glial processes more easily that Hebbian plasticity since its

induction usually involves a wide-scale activity change (rather than activation at

single inputs). Indeed, glial and cell adhesion molecules are emerging as

important players in the HSP process (Thalhammer and Cingolani, 2014).

Towards the end of the scaling pathway, stargazin, an AMPA auxiliary subunit,

has been shown to play a crucial role in AMPAR trafficking in the synapse upon

induction of HSP (Louros et al., 2014). Stargazin is known to regulate the

delivery of AMPARs to the synapse. It is up-regulated upon activity blockade,

whereupon it is phosphorylated to promote scaling.

Alongside these major signalling players, numerous other molecules have been

shown to influence scaling, including BDNF (Rutherford et al., 1998), MSK1

(Corrêa et al., 2012), Beta-3 integrins (Cingolani et al., 2008), STEP61 (Jang et al.,

2015) and MHC1 (Goddard et al., 2007). It is clear that, as for Hebbian plasticity,

HSP is an extremely complex process which requires many layers of control for

its successful expression.

1.5.1.2 Retinoic acid dependent up-scaling

In 1998, just subsequent to the first description of HSP (Turrigiano et al., 1998),

changes in the accumulation of AMPA receptors following chronic increases or

decreases of synaptic signalling were described (O’Brien et al., 1998). When APV,

an NMDA receptor antagonist, was applied in conjunction with an activity

blocker for 72 hrs to eliminate synaptic signals, AMPA receptor levels increased

in the synapses. Subsequent work using this protocol of NMDA receptor

Page 38: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

20

blockade elucidated that this form of HSP is mechanistically different to the one

described in the previous section (Ju et al., 2004; Sutton et al., 2004, 2006; Aoto

et al., 2008; Sarti et al., 2012). The reason for this is that not only is activity-

dependent glutamate release inhibited, but also the activation of post-synaptic

NMDA receptors by spontaneous glutamate release (miniature synaptic

transmission) is prevented. The loss of miniature signalling causes an up-

regulation of translation in the dendrite, accompanied by up-scaling of synaptic

strengths, which can be observed only an hour after application of the combined

drugs (Sutton et al., 2004, 2006). In contrast, synaptic scaling caused by only

action potential blockade can take 24 hours or more (Sutton et al., 2006). This

fast form of HSP has been found to rely on a newly discovered function for all-

trans retinoic acid (RA), a signalling molecule previously known for its role as a

morphogen during development (Aoto et al., 2008; Chen et al., 2014). Two hours

of RA application induces synaptic scaling, and scaling caused by TTX + APV is

blocked by inhibitors of RA signalling (Aoto et al., 2008; Sarti et al., 2012). These

two pathways – RA-mediated and non-RA mediated – remain mechanistically

distinct even at the timescales where both are evident, i.e. at more than 24 hours

of treatment, at which time TTX-mediated synaptic scaling is not affected by RA

inhibition, but TTX + APV scaling is abolished (Soden and Chen, 2010; Wang et

al., 2011).

Blockade of NMDA receptors inhibits Ca2+ entry thus lowering Ca2+ levels in the

dendrite, which has been found to be a crucial step for the up-regulation of RA

synthesis (Pawlak et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2011). This difference in Ca2+

signalling may explain why TTX + APV co-ops RA-mediated HSP, whilst TTX

alone does not. As mentioned in the previous section, Ca2+ signalling in the soma,

rather than the dendrites, was important for TTX-induced HSP (Ibata et al.,

2008). Calcium concentration is sensed by calciuneurin, a Ca2+-dependent

phosphotase, which then promotes RA synthesis (Arendt et al., 2015). In addition

to calcium sensing, RA-synthesis and GluA1 synthesis also happen locally at

Page 39: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

21

dendrites (Ju et al., 2004; Maghsoodi et al., 2008). This could give a vital insight

into the different spatial scales at which HSP is seen in the literature. Non-RA

mediated HSP, which is sensed at a cell-wide level, could co-opt scaling at all

synapses simultaneously, whereas RA-mediated, which senses activity and

initiates signalling mechanisms at a local level, could act on much more

restricted spatial scales.

I have referred to this type of HSP as the ‘RA-dependent form’, and differentiated

it from the previously described non-RA dependent form. It is probable that it is

not only the RA component, but the whole signalling cascade which differs

between these two types. Much less is known however about the other

components. One better explored aspect involves protein synthesis. The scaling

response requires both eEF2 (Sutton et al., 2007), and FMRP (Soden and Chen,

2010) which are both involved in the control of translation in dendrites. The

requirement for protein translation and transcription will be explored more fully

in section 1.5.2 but the de novo synthesis of proteins is an important part of

scaling expression.

As with the previously described forms of HSP, RA-dependent scaling is effected

by AMPAR insertion into the synapse. Whereas GluA2 seems to be the major

subunit player in non-RA dependent HSP, the RA-dependent form is instantiated

by GluA2-lacking receptors, likely GluA1 homomers (Ju et al., 2004; Thiagarajan

et al., 2005; Shepherd et al., 2006; Sutton et al., 2006). These receptors are

dendritically synthesised which may account for the fast dynamics of this type of

scaling, since the receptors do not have to be newly transcribed and transported

from the soma (Ju et al., 2004). Some recent work has shed light on how the

synthesis of AMPAR subunits is controlled through interactions with micro-RNAs

(miRNAs). These short lengths of RNA have received great attention in recent

years for their regulatory roles in mRNA translation. Two different miRNAs have

been identified which modulate AMPAR translation in opposing directions

Page 40: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

22

during synaptic scaling – miR-92a which suppresses GluA1 synthesis and is

down-regulated upon activity blockade (Letellier et al., 2014), and miR124 which

suppresses GluA2 synthesis during activity blockade, leading to the formation of

GluA2-lacking AMPARs (Hou et al., 2015).

1.5.1.3 Down-scaling

HSP would be of limited use if it could only implement scaling in one direction.

Indeed, scaling downwards in response to over-activity is also known in the

literature (O’Brien et al., 1998; Turrigiano et al., 1998; Goold and Nicoll, 2010;

Hou et al., 2011). It is worth noting however that there are many fewer studies

than for up-scaling, and the vast majority of these are in primary cultured

neurons, instead of more the physiologically relevant systems of slices or in vivo.

This may be partly due to the increased experimental challenges of chronically

increasing activity (though see (Corrêa et al., 2012) for the use of environmental

enrichment as an activity increase paradigm), but it could also represent that the

system is not symmetric with regards to the methods used to implement up-

scaling versus down-scaling, and so down-scaling is harder to observe.

That down-scaling is a qualitatively different process to up-scaling is shown by

the different pathways involved. For instance, CaMKII, fundamental for the

expression of up-scaling, is not required for down-scaling (Goold and Nicoll,

2010; Hou et al., 2011). A change in calcium level is still the most likely means by

which activity levels are sensed, as shown by the necessity of various neuronal

components which control intra-cellular calcium, including NMDA receptors and

voltage-gated calcium channels (Goold and Nicoll, 2010; Hou et al., 2011). Other

calcium-binding molecules must therefore take the calcium-sensing role that

CaMKII played in up-scaling. Indeed, different members of the CaMK signalling

cascade, most prominently CaMKK (which activates CaMKIV and CaMKI), are

necessary for the expression of down-scaling (Goold and Nicoll, 2010).

Page 41: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

23

Once over-activity has been sensed, a process to decrease synaptic strength must

be enacted. Here the immediate early gene Arc plays an important role (Bateup

et al., 2013a; Korb et al., 2013). As well as acting as a marker for recent activity,

Arc is known to play a role in LTD by stimulating the endocytosis of glutamate

receptors (Bramham et al., 2008). It is unsurprising that it also proves essential

for the weakening of synapses in response to chronic over-activity, since AMPA

removal is responsible for the functional post-synaptic change after downwards

HSP (O’Brien et al., 1998). Arc is up-regulated after increased activity (Bateup et

al., 2013b), and also acts to down-regulate transcription of GluA1 (Korb et al.,

2013). Arc’s well-documented fast induction after synaptic activity (for which it

has become known as an immediate-early gene) may have another consequence;

that down-scaling is induced at shorter timescales than up-scaling. Using

optogenetic methods to increase activity of single cells, the removal of GluA1

receptors in spines was seen to take place as early as 30 minutes into the light

treatment (Hou et al., 2011). Other studies report strong effects after only a few

hours of treatment (Bateup et al., 2013a, 2013b). Surprisingly however, Arc KO

neurons are still capable of synaptic down-scaling, possibly implying that a

number of parallel redundant pathways can achieve this result (Shepherd et al.,

2006).

Finally, a host of other molecular players have also been shown to be necessary

for synaptic down-scaling, including PP1 I-2 (Siddoway et al., 2013), MeCP2 (Qiu

et al., 2012; Zhong et al., 2012) and components of the Plk pathway and CDK

(Seeburg and Sheng, 2008; Seeburg et al., 2008) (for a full review of molecular

pathways see (Siddoway et al., 2014)).

1.5.2 The involvement of proteins in HSP

Proteins are fundamental building blocks of cellular processes, and plasticity is

no exception. As with Hebbian plasticity, de novo protein synthesis seems to be

necessary to maintain HSP for extended periods of time. Synthesis of GluA1

Page 42: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

24

occurs locally in dendrites in response to RA-mediated HSP (Ju et al., 2004;

Sutton et al., 2004, 2006). FMRP, an important player in controlling levels of

protein synthesis, is required for this process (Soden and Chen, 2010). Non-RA

mediated HSP however requires not just translation but also transcription –

reinforcing the theory already expounded in this introduction that the non-RA

form is a global cell wide process, whereas the RA-form is more local (Ibata et al.,

2008). Similarly, global downscaling required not just translation but also

transcription (Goold and Nicoll, 2010). These findings reinforce the fundamental

role new proteins play in the expression and maintenance of HSP.

1.5.3 HSP in vivo

The majority of work on HSP has been conducted in culture systems, either in

primary neuron cultures (Turrigiano et al., 1998) or in organotypic slices

(Karmarkar and Buonomano, 2006). This is due to the increased tractability of

the in vitro system, where chemical manipulations, and their molecular and

physiological effects, are much more easily controlled and assessed than in the

live animal. In vitro systems have allowed huge strides to be made in

understanding how HSP works using these systems. An increasing number of

studies however have now addressed the question of how HSP is expressed in

intact nervous systems.

Many of the in vitro studies on the mechanisms and expression of HSP have been

borne out through in vivo studies. Multiplicative synaptic scaling has been seen

in various sensory cortices after deprivation, including visual (Desai et al., 2002;

Goel and Lee, 2007; Keck et al., 2013) and somatosensory (Greenhill et al., 2015),

as well as in other regions such as the spinal cord (Knogler et al., 2010; Garcia-

Bereguiain et al., 2013). As a counter example, synaptic scaling was absent after

in vivo TTX infusion in the hippocampus (Echegoyen et al., 2007).

Page 43: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

25

One potentially confounding factor for drawing a universal conclusion about HSP

in vivo is that the type and extent expressed is highly influenced by

developmental age (Desai et al., 2002; Knogler et al., 2010). In sensory cortices,

as well as other parts of the brain, the neurons progress though periods with

differing capacities of plasticity expression. The peak, normally during infancy or

childhood, is known as the critical period, and is the time when much of the

framework for the sensory processing is established. Without activity during this

period, the neurons can never recover their optimal functioning into adulthood

(Wiesel and Hubel, 1963). This inability to recover from deprivation suggests

that homeostasis is suppressed in some areas during the critical period –

possibly because compensatory mechanisms could hinder the development of

acuity. Indeed at the retinocollicular synapse, mice without normal retinal waves

throughout development (due to deletion of a critical protein) have larger

receptive fields, compensating for the lower peak responses (Chandrasekaran et

al., 2007). In the visual cortex, studies from the Turrigiano lab found that

homeostasis in layer 4 visual cortical neurons was prominent in early life (Desai

et al., 2002; Maffei et al., 2004) but was switched off by P21 (Maffei et al., 2006),

indicating that by the beginning of the visual critical period homeostasis is

suppressed. In upper layers of the cortex however, plasticity remains prominent

through the critical period (Maffei and Turrigiano, 2008) and into adulthood

(Goel and Lee, 2007). An interesting study which examined the length of time

needed to induce homeostatic responses at different developmental ages in the

hippocampus found that in early life only 15 hours of activity shift was needed,

but later in development more than 40 hours was required for HSP induction

(Huupponen et al., 2007). These differences may reflect that fact that throughout

development, the subunit composition of receptors is known to change, altering

the functional properties of the circuit. One example is the NMDA receptor, a

cation channel which acts as a coincidence detector for pre- and post- synaptic

activity and thus is vital for circuit refinement and Hebbian plasticity. This

Page 44: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

26

receptor has a developmental switch, from being enriched in the subunit

GluN2B, which is highly calcium permeable and has a prolonged channel

opening, to GluN2A, which with its decreased calcium permeability reduces the

capacity for plasticity (Paoletti et al., 2013). These differences in synaptic

receptors may make different types of homeostasis more or less likely,

depending on the developmental stage they manifest. What is clear is that HSP,

and indeed other complementary mechanisms of homeostasis, exist in a complex

interplay throughout different periods of the lifetime of an animal.

1.6 Structural changes accompanying plasticity

As is detailed above, much has been elucidated regarding the molecular

pathways and functional expression of HSP. An area which is still to be explored

is the structural consequences of HSP for the dendritic spines and the neuron.

The structure of a neuron – its physical size and shape – is strongly influential

with regards to its function. The high degree of specialisation seen in different

neuron subtypes (consider the extreme differences between pyramidal cells,

Purkinje cells and motor neurons), shows how precisely adapted different

neuron types are for their roles. Although there is undoubtedly a large genetic

component, structural parameters such as dendrite branching, spine number or

axon diameter are not fixed but can undergo activity and time-dependent

changes throughout development and into adulthood.

In this thesis I will focus on structural changes to the dendritic arbour, since

there is a wider scope for plasticity in the dendrites than the axon, and the

dendrite and spines provide a visual read-out to predict downstream responses

to activity. There is some evidence for structural changes to the axon upon

homeostatic plasticity induction but this is outside the scope of this work

(Yamahachi et al., 2009). Dendritic structural changes strongly influence the

Page 45: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

27

function of the cell by changing the electrical and biochemical properties of its

inputs, and thus its firing pattern. A simplistic example is that if a cell develops

more dendritic spines it has increased input and thus will be more likely to fire

(although this can of course be counterbalanced by other type of modifications

such as threshold changes). A more subtle example occurs at the level of a single

spine, where by increasing the spine volume there is more room for both

receptors (such as AMPARs) and also organelles such as endoplasmic reticulum,

which could enhance signalling at the synapse and so strengthen it. Hebbian

plasticity is known to cause structural changes, for both LTP (Matsuzaki et al.,

2004) and LTD (Ramiro-Cortés and Israely, 2013). Much less is known about

structural correlates with regards to homeostatic plasticity. In particular, it

remains to be discovered whether indeed structural changes are a hallmark of

HSP, and if so whether they mirror the changes seen in Hebbian plasticity or

whether they employ parallel mechanisms.

Much of the evidence for structural changes following HSP comes not from a

mammalian system but from Drosophila, a classic model for homeostasis studies.

Activity blockade throughout development causes large structural re-

organisations, including increases in dendritic arbour size and complexity

(Tripodi et al., 2008) and a larger synaptic glomerulus (Kremer et al., 2010).

Drosophila synapses are not located on dendritic spines however, so the

adaptations seen in may be different to those in a chordate system. In

mammalian neurons, there is ample evidence that the number of synaptic

connections changes after HSP. Here the developmental stage of activity

deprivation determines the structural adaptations, with decreased synapses if

deprivation occurs before synaptogenesis, but increased synapses if it occurs

after (Stroemer et al., 1995; Burrone et al., 2002; Kirov et al., 2004; Zuo et al.,

2005a; Arendt et al., 2013). Complementing this, synaptic elimination follows

chronic increases in activity (Goold and Nicoll, 2010). However, see (Wallace and

Bear, 2004; Thiagarajan et al., 2005) for experiments which result in unchanged

Page 46: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

28

or decreased spine density following reduced activity. Interestingly, spine

turnover can still occur even throughout chronic blockade of activity (Yasumatsu

et al., 2008).

Not only spine number but also the structure of spines themselves can change

with shifts in activity, although there is conflicting evidence over the dynamics of

this process. Sensory deprivation in vivo and chemical blockade of activity have

been reported to cause spine volume increases, accompanied by corresponding

increases in the size of the pre-synaptic terminals (Murthy et al., 2001; Wallace

and Bear, 2004; Keck et al., 2013). This affect was seen to be reversible after re-

exposure to activity (Wallace and Bear, 2004). On the other hand, various studies

have reported no change to spine volume following chemical manipulations or

single synapses activity loss (Yasumatsu et al., 2008; Béïque et al., 2011).

Presumably, these differences relate to the induction protocols used in the

specific experiments.

The individual morphology of the spines also influences function. Spines are

traditionally divided into 4 different classes; mushroom, stubby, thin and

filopodia, with mushroom spines being the most mature and filopodia the most

immature (Harris et al., 1992). In a normal system, a mixture of these different

types is present. Throughout development the distribution of spine types evolves

to the mature state. Certain neurodevelopmental disorders are known to skew

this distribution. For example, loss of the Fragile X Mental Retardation protein 1

(FMRP1), which in humans causes the mental retardation disorder Fragile X

Syndrome, leads to an over-proliferation of thin spines and filopodia (i.e.

immature spines) in cortical and hippocampal regions (Irwin et al., 2000).

Conversely, mutations in MeCP2, a transcriptional regulator, which in humans

causes Rett syndrome, leads to increased spine volume (Xu et al., 2014). This

inability to maintain the correct balance of spine types may well be causal for the

cognitive impairments which accompany these disorders. It is currently

Page 47: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

29

unknown whether the expression of HSP changes the distribution of spine types

in a neural system (although it is interesting to note that both FMRP and MeCP2

have been implicated in synaptic scaling (Soden and Chen, 2010; Qiu et al.,

2012)). It is clear that for this important topic, more work is needed to fully

establish the role that structural modifications play in homeostatic synaptic

plasticity.

1.7 Interplay between Hebbian and Homeostatic plasticity

Since the discovery of plasticity, the majority of research has been conducted in

reduced systems such as primary cultures or brain slices. These allow for precise

control of biological parameters such as activity, connectivity and molecular

signalling, allowing isolation of the crucial elements of plasticity without external

confounding factors. Through this approach we have gleaned a vast and detailed

knowledge of molecular, cellular and network level pathways of plasticity

processes. Studying single types of plasticity in isolated systems does, however,

ignore the fact that in vivo these processes will take place on a background of

many integrated activity events. The way in which different processes – for

example homeostatic plasticity and Hebbian plasticity – affect each other is of

great interest to understand how intact nervous systems integrate many

different and sometimes conflicting sources of information.

Hebbian plasticity shares many of the traits already described for HSP.

Mechanistically, Hebbian plasticity is dependent on signalling mechanisms

triggered by synaptic activity. These normally involve calcium influx into the

spine through NMDA receptors. Changes in synaptic strength are effected by

changing the number of AMPA receptors in the post-synaptic density. AMPA

receptors may be ‘held’ in preparation either in vesicles in the spine or dendrite,

or in the extra-synaptic membrane. Upon receiving the LTP stimulus, these

Page 48: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

30

receptors can move into the post-synaptic density, increasing the electrical

conductance of the spine. An increase in synaptic strength is also correlated with

an increase in spine volume, such that under normal conditions there is a linear

relationship between the strength of the spine and its volume (Matsuzaki et al.,

2004).

It is not known whether the increase of conductance seen after homeostatic

AMPA insertion is functionally equivalent to classical Hebbian LTP/LTD. It may

instead represent a separate mechanism, such as a ‘priming’ of the synapses to

allow them to respond more strongly to stimuli in the future. Since there is a

large overlap between the endpoints of both Hebbian plasticity and HSP, it is still

to be elucidated whether the two interfere with each other or if the signalling

mechanisms are distinct. Theoretical studies have predicted that the two

processes are separable (Toyoizumi et al., 2014), and indeed that HSP may

influence the stability of Hebbian plasticity (Rabinowitch and Segev, 2006). Only

a limited number of studies have sought to address this question experimentally,

using a variety of HSP induction mechanisms, including global activity blockade

(Arendt et al., 2013), NMDA receptor blockade (Félix-Oliveira et al., 2014) and

single-spine inactivation (Lee et al., 2010). These different protocols will each

induce a mechanistically different type of HSP as detailed previously in this

introduction. However, in all cases LTP is reported to be stronger after HSP,

whether it is induced through electrical stimulation of axon bundles (Arendt et

al., 2013; Félix-Oliveira et al., 2014) or by single-spine stimulation through local

glutamate release (Lee et al., 2010). The mechanistic reasons for this difference

are related to the NMDA receptor content of the synapses after HSP. There is

reported to be an increase in silent (i.e. AMPAR-lacking) synapses after

prolonged activity blockade, but no change in the subunit composition of the

receptor (Arendt et al., 2013). However, a different activity reduction protocol

demonstrated an increase in the concentration of the NMDAR subunits GluN1

and GluN2B (Lee et al., 2010). GluN2B is known to bind CamKII with high affinity

Page 49: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

31

and have higher calcium permeability than its counterpart GluN2A (Barria and

Malinow, 2005; Paoletti et al., 2013). This switch therefore seems to ‘prime’ the

system for more activation following the LTP stimulus, increasing the input to

the downstream cell.

There are many unknowns still to be investigated in the domain of Hebbian

plasticity and HSP interactions. These include the structural consequences for

spines, the long-term plasticity dynamics, and which spines in a population will

be more prone to express plasticity. We will address these topics experimentally

in the remainder of this thesis.

1.8 Outline of work in the thesis

In this thesis, we examine how the structural correlates of HSP relate to the

functional changes that have been described. In chapter 2, we report the

structural changes which occur at dendritic spines that are induced by chronic

blockade of activity, and the corresponding functional changes that accompany

them. We assess the ways in which the structure changes, and elaborate on what

the results imply for homeostatic plasticity theory. We test how the system can

recover after activity blockade is removed, both structurally and functionally, to

see whether flexibility is retained after HSP. We finally expose the slices to

chronic increases of activity to assess structural and functional changes.

In chapter 3, we investigate how HSP and Hebbian plasticity co-exist together.

For this we induce LTP after the expression of HSP, and use a structural read-out

of plasticity to draw conclusions about the capability of a system to retain

information after HSP. We investigate what structural parameters determine

how the capacity of the spines to express LTP, and how this is altered by the

expression of HSP. We also use different strengths of induction to determine

Page 50: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

32

what the threshold for plasticity is under different conditions. Finally we

examine the effect of HSP on neighbouring spine plasticity.

Together, the experiments in this thesis help to shed light on how HSP can affect

the structural parameters of neurons, and the long-term consequences this will

have for the functioning of the neural circuit.

Page 51: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

33

1.9 Bibliography

Aoto J, Nam CI, Poon MM, Ting P, Chen L (2008) Synaptic Signaling by All-Trans Retinoic Acid in Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity. Neuron 60:308–320.

Arendt KL, Sarti F, Chen L (2013) Chronic inactivation of a neural circuit enhances LTP by inducing silent synapse formation. J Neurosci 33:2087–2096.

Arendt KL, Zhang Z, Ganesan S, Hintze M, Shin MM, Tang Y, Cho A, Graef IA, Chen L (2015) Calcineurin mediates homeostatic synaptic plasticity by regulating retinoic acid synthesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci:201510239.

Atallah B V., Scanziani M (2009) Instantaneous Modulation of Gamma Oscillation Frequency by Balancing Excitation with Inhibition. Neuron 62:566–577.

Axelsson J, Thesleff S (1959) A study of supersensitivity in denervated mammalian skeletal muscle. J Physiol 147:178–193.

Barria A, Malinow R (2005) NMDA receptor subunit composition controls synaptic plasticity by regulating binding to CaMKII. Neuron 48:289–301.

Barth AL, Gerkin RC, Dean KL (2004) Alteration of neuronal firing properties after in vivo experience in a FosGFP transgenic mouse. J Neurosci 24:6466–6475.

Bateup HS, Denefrio CL, Johnson CA, Saulnier JL, Sabatini BL (2013a) Temporal dynamics of a homeostatic pathway controlling neural network activity. Front Mol Neurosci 6:28.

Bateup HS, Johnson CA, Denefrio CL, Saulnier JL, Kornacker K, Sabatini BL (2013b) Excitatory/inhibitory synaptic imbalance leads to hippocampal hyperexcitability in mouse models of tuberous sclerosis. Neuron 78:510–522.

Béïque JJ-C, Na Y, Kuhl D, Worley PF, Huganir RL (2011) Arc-dependent synapse-specific homeostatic plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108:816–821.

Benevento LA, Bakkum BW, Cohen RS (1995) Gamma-Aminobutyric-Acid and Somatostatin Immunoreactivity in the Visual-Cortex of Normal and Dark-Reared Rats. Brain Res 689:172–182.

Bienenstock EL, Cooper LN, Munro PW (1982) Theory for the development of neuron selectivity: orientation specificity and binocular interaction in visual cortex. J Neurosci 2:32–48.

Page 52: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

34

Bliss T, Lømo T (1973) Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol:331–356.

Bramham CR, Worley PF, Moore MJ, Guzowski JF (2008) The immediate early gene arc/arg3.1: regulation, mechanisms, and function. J Neurosci 28:11760–11767.

Branco T, Staras K, Darcy KJ, Goda Y (2008) Local Dendritic Activity Sets Release Probability at Hippocampal Synapses. Neuron 59:475–485.

Breton J-D, Stuart GJ (2009) Loss of sensory input increases the intrinsic excitability of layer 5 pyramidal neurons in rat barrel cortex. J Physiol 587:5107–5119.

Brown GL (1937) The actions of acetylcholine on denervated mammalian and frog’s muscle. J Physiol 89:438–461.

Burrone J, O’Byrne M, Murthy VN (2002) Multiple forms of synaptic plasticity triggered by selective suppression of activity in individual neurons. Nature 420.

Cannon WB (1932) The Wisdom of the Body. W.W. Norton & Company.

Chandrasekaran AR, Shah RD, Crair MC (2007) Developmental homeostasis of mouse retinocollicular synapses. J Neurosci 27:1746–1755.

Chang MC, Park JM, Pelkey KA, Grabenstatter HL, Xu D, Linden DJ, Sutula TP, McBain CJ, Worley PF (2010) Narp regulates homeostatic scaling of excitatory synapses on parvalbumin-expressing interneurons. Nat Neurosci 13:1090–1097.

Chen L, Lau AG, Sarti F (2014) Synaptic retinoic acid signaling and homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Neuropharmacology 78:3–12.

Cingolani LA, Thalhammer A, Yu LMY, Catalano M, Ramos T, Colicos MA, Goda Y (2008) Activity-Dependent Regulation of Synaptic AMPA Receptor Composition and Abundance by β3 Integrins. Neuron 58:749–762.

Clopath C, Büsing L, Vasilaki E, Gerstner W (2010) Connectivity reflects coding: a model of voltage-based STDP with homeostasis. Nat Neurosci 13:344–352.

Corner M, Ramakers G (1991) Spontaneous Bioelectric Activity as Both Dependent and Independent Variable in Cortical Maturation Chronic Tetrodotoxin versus Picrotoxin Effects. Ann New York Acad Sci.

Page 53: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

35

Corrêa SAL, Hunter CJ, Palygin O, Wauters SC, Martin KJ, McKenzie C, McKelvey K, Morris RGM, Pankratov Y, Arthur JSC, Frenguelli BG (2012) MSK1 regulates homeostatic and experience-dependent synaptic plasticity. J Neurosci 32:13039–13051.

Cudmore RH, Fronzaroli-Molinieres L, Giraud P, Debanne D (2010) Spike-time precision and network synchrony are controlled by the homeostatic regulation of the D-type potassium current. J Neurosci 30:12885–12895.

Davis GW (2006) Homeostatic control of neural activity: from phenomenology to molecular design. Annu Rev Neurosci 29:307–323.

Davis GW (2013) Homeostatic signaling and the stabilization of neural function. Neuron 80:718–728.

Davis GW, DiAntonio A, Petersen SA, Goodman CS (1998) Postsynaptic PKA controls quantal size and reveals a retrograde signal that regulates presynaptic transmitter release in Drosophila. Neuron 20:305–315.

Davis GW, Müller M (2014) Homeostatic Control of Presynaptic Neurotransmitter Release. Annu Rev Physiol:1–20.

Desai NS, Cudmore RH, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG (2002) Critical periods for experience-dependent synaptic scaling in visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 5:783–789.

Desai NS, Rutherford LC, Turrigiano GG (1999) Plasticity in the intrinsic excitability of cortical pyramidal neurons. Nat Neurosci 2:515–520.

Diering GHH, Gustina ASS, Huganir RLL (2014) PKA-GluA1 Coupling via AKAP5 Controls AMPA Receptor Phosphorylation and Cell-Surface Targeting during Bidirectional Homeostatic Plasticity. Neuron 845:1–16.

Echegoyen J, Neu A, Graber KD, Soltesz I (2007) Homeostatic plasticity studied using in vivo hippocampal activity-blockade: synaptic scaling, intrinsic plasticity and age-dependence. PLoS One 2:e700.

Félix-Oliveira A, Dias RB, Colino-Oliveira M, Rombo DM, Sebastião AM (2014) Homeostatic plasticity induced by brief activity deprivation enhances long-term potentiation in the mature rat hippocampus. J Neurophysiol 112:3012–3022.

Fong M-F, Newman JP, Potter SM, Wenner P (2015) Upward synaptic scaling is dependent on neurotransmission rather than spiking. Nat Commun 6:6339.

Frank CA, Kennedy MJ, Goold CP, Marek KW, Davis GW (2006) Mechanisms

Page 54: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

36

Underlying the Rapid Induction and Sustained Expression of Synaptic Homeostasis. Neuron 52:663–677.

Frank CA, Pielage J, Davis GW (2009) A Presynaptic Homeostatic Signaling System Composed of the Eph Receptor, Ephexin, Cdc42, and CaV2.1 Calcium Channels. Neuron 61:556–569.

Gainey M a, Hurvitz-Wolff JR, Lambo ME, Turrigiano GG (2009) Synaptic scaling requires the GluR2 subunit of the AMPA receptor. J Neurosci 29:6479–6489.

Garcia-Bereguiain MA, Gonzalez-Islas C, Lindsly C, Butler E, Hill AW, Wenner P (2013) In vivo synaptic scaling is mediated by GluA2-lacking AMPA receptors in the embryonic spinal cord. J Neurosci 33:6791–6799.

Goddard CA, Butts DA, Shatz CJ (2007) Regulation of CNS synapses by neuronal MHC class I. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:6828–6833.

Goel A, Lee H-K (2007) Persistence of Experience-Induced Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity through Adulthood in Superficial Layers of Mouse Visual Cortex. J Neurosci 27:6692–6700.

Goold CP, Nicoll RA (2010) Single-cell optogenetic excitation drives homeostatic synaptic depression. Neuron 68:512–528.

Greenhill SD, Ranson A, Fox K (2015) Hebbian and Homeostatic Plasticity Mechanisms in Regular Spiking and Intrinsic Bursting Cells of Cortical Layer 5. Neuron 88:539–552.

Hardingham N, Dachtler J, Fox K (2013) The role of nitric oxide in pre-synaptic plasticity and homeostasis. Front Cell Neurosci 7:190.

Harris KM, Jensen FE, Tsao B (1992) Three-Dimensional Structure of Dendritic Spines and Synapses in Rat Hippocampus (CA1) at Postnatal Day 15 and Adult Ages : Implications for the Maturation of Synaptic Physiology and Long-term Potentiation. J Neurosci 12.

Hartman KN, Pal SK, Burrone J, Murthy VN (2006) Activity-dependent regulation of inhibitory synaptic transmission in hippocampal neurons. Nat Neurosci 9:642–649.

Hebb D (1949) The Organization of Behavior. New York : Wiley.

Hendry SHC, Jones EG (1986) Reduction in number of immunostained GABAergic neurones in deprived-eye dominance columns of monkey area 17. Nature 320:750–753.

Page 55: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

37

Holopainen I., Lauren H. (2003) Neuronal activity regulates gabaa receptor subunit expression in organotypic hippocampal slice cultures. Neuroscience 118:967–974.

Hou Q, Gilbert J, Man H-Y (2011) Homeostatic regulation of AMPA receptor trafficking and degradation by light-controlled single-synaptic activation. Neuron 72:806–818.

Hou Q, Ruan H, Gilbert J, Wang G, Ma Q, Yao W-D, Man H-Y (2015) MicroRNA miR124 is required for the expression of homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Nat Commun 6:10045.

Hou Q, Zhang D, Jarzylo L, Huganir RL, Man H-Y (2008) Homeostatic regulation of AMPA receptor expression at single hippocampal synapses. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:775–780.

Huupponen J, Molchanova SM, Taira T, Lauri SE (2007) Susceptibility for homeostatic plasticity is down-regulated in parallel with maturation of the rat hippocampal synaptic circuitry. J Physiol 581:505–514.

Ibata K, Sun Q, Turrigiano GG (2008) Rapid Synaptic Scaling Induced by Changes in Postsynaptic Firing. Neuron 57:819–826.

Irwin SA, Galvez R, Greenough WT (2000) Dendritic spine structural anomalies in fragile-X mental retardation syndrome. Cereb Cortex 10:1038–1044.

Isaac JTR, Ashby M, McBain CJ (2007) The Role of the GluR2 Subunit in AMPA Receptor Function and Synaptic Plasticity. Neuron 54:859–871.

Jakawich SK, Nasser HB, Strong MJ, McCartney AJ, Perez AS, Rakesh N, Carruthers CJL, Sutton MA (2010) Local Presynaptic Activity Gates Homeostatic Changes in Presynaptic Function Driven by Dendritic BDNF Synthesis. Neuron 68:1143–1158.

Jang S-S, Royston SE, Xu J, Cavaretta JP, Vest MO, Lee KY, Lee S, Jeong HG, Lombroso PJ, Chung HJ (2015) Regulation of STEP61 and tyrosine-phosphorylation of NMDA and AMPA receptors during homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Mol Brain 8:55.

Jensen TP, Buckby LE, Empson RM (2009) Reduced expression of the “fast” calcium transporter PMCA2a during homeostatic plasticity. Mol Cell Neurosci 41:364–372.

Ju W, Morishita W, Tsui J, Gaietta G, Deerinck TJ, Adams SR, Garner CC, Tsien RY, Ellisman MH, Malenka RC (2004) Activity-dependent regulation of dendritic synthesis and trafficking of AMPA receptors. Nat Neurosci 7:244–

Page 56: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

38

253.

Kandel ER, Tauc L (1965) Heterosynaptic facilitation in neurones of the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia depilans. J Physiol 181:28–47.

Kaneko M, Stellwagen D, Malenka RC, Stryker MP (2008) Tumor Necrosis Factor-a Mediates One Component of Competitive, Experience-Dependent Plasticity in Developing Visual Cortex. Neuron 58:673–680.

Karmarkar UR, Buonomano D V (2006) Different forms of homeostatic plasticity are engaged with distinct temporal profiles. Eur J Neurosci 23:1575–1584.

Keck T, Keller GB, Jacobsen RI, Eysel UT, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M (2013) Synaptic scaling and homeostatic plasticity in the mouse visual cortex in vivo. Neuron 80:327–334.

Kilman V, van Rossum MCW, Turrigiano GG (2002) Activity deprivation reduces miniature IPSC amplitude by decreasing the number of postsynaptic GABA(A) receptors clustered at neocortical synapses. J Neurosci 22:1328–1337.

Kim J, Tsien RW (2008) Synapse-specific adaptations to inactivity in hippocampal circuits achieve homeostatic gain control while dampening network reverberation. Neuron 58:925–937.

Kirov SA, Goddard CA, Harris KM (2004) Age-dependence in the homeostatic upregulation of hippocampal dendritic spine number during blocked synaptic transmission. Neuropharmacology 47:640–648.

Knogler LD, Liao M, Drapeau P (2010) Synaptic scaling and the development of a motor network. J Neurosci 30:8871–8881.

Korb E, Wilkinson CL, Delgado RN, Lovero KL, Finkbeiner S (2013) Arc in the nucleus regulates PML-dependent GluA1 transcription and homeostatic plasticity. Nat Neurosci 16:874–883.

Kremer MC, Christiansen F, Leiss F, Paehler M, Knapek S, Andlauer TFM, Forstner F, Kloppenburg P, Sigrist SJ, Tavosanis G (2010) Structural long-term changes at mushroom body input synapses. Curr Biol 20:1938–1944.

Lee K, Royston SE, Vest MO, Ley DJ, Lee S, Bolton EC, Chung H (2015) N -methyl-D-aspartate receptors mediate activity-dependent down-regulation of potassium channel genes during the expression of homeostatic intrinsic plasticity. Mol Brain 8:4.

Lee KJ, Queenan BN, Rozeboom AM, Bellmore R, Lim ST, Vicini S, Pak DTS (2013)

Page 57: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

39

Mossy fiber-CA3 synapses mediate homeostatic plasticity in mature hippocampal neurons. Neuron 77:99–114.

Lee KY, Chung HJ (2014) NMDA receptors and L-type voltage-gated Ca2+ channels mediate the expression of bidirectional homeostatic intrinsic plasticity in cultured hippocampal neurons. Neuroscience 277:610–623.

Lee M-C, Yasuda R, Ehlers MD (2010) Metaplasticity at Single Glutamatergic Synapses. Neuron 66:859–870.

Letellier M, Elramah S, Mondin M, Soula A, Penn A, Choquet D, Landry M, Thoumine O, Favereaux A (2014) miR-92a regulates expression of synaptic GluA1-containing AMPA receptors during homeostatic scaling. Nat Neurosci 17:1040–1042.

Lindskog M, Li L, Groth RD, Poburko D, Thiagarajan TC, Han X, Tsien RW (2010) Postsynaptic GluA1 enables acute retrograde enhancement of presynaptic function to coordinate adaptation to synaptic inactivity. Pnas 107:21806–21811.

Lissin D V, Gomperts SN, Carroll RC, Christine CW, Kalman D, Kitamura M, Hardy S, Nicoll RA, Malenka RC, von Zastrow M (1998) Activity differentially regulates the surface expression of synaptic AMPA and NMDA glutamate receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:7097–7102.

Litwin-Kumar A, Doiron B (2014) Formation and maintenance of neuronal assemblies through synaptic plasticity. Nat Commun 5:5319.

Liu Z, Golowasch J, Marder E, Abbott LF (1998) A model neuron with activity-dependent conductances regulated by multiple calcium sensors. J Neurosci 18:2309–2320.

Louros SR, Hooks BM, Litvina L, Carvalho AL, Chen C (2014) A role for stargazin in experience-dependent plasticity. Cell Rep 7:1614–1625.

Lynch GS, Dunwiddie T, Gribkoff V (1977) Heterosynaptic depression: a postsynaptic correlate of long-term potentiation. Nature 266:737–739.

Maffei A, Nataraj K, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG (2006) Potentiation of cortical inhibition by visual deprivation. Nature 443:81–84.

Maffei A, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG (2004) Selective reconfiguration of layer 4 visual cortical circuitry by visual deprivation. Nat Neurosci 7:1353–1359.

Maffei A, Turrigiano GG (2008) Multiple modes of network homeostasis in visual cortical layer 2/3. J Neurosci 28:4377–4384.

Page 58: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

40

Maghsoodi B, Poon MM, Nam CI, Aoto J, Ting P, Chen L (2008) Retinoic acid regulates RARalpha-mediated control of translation in dendritic RNA granules during homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:16015–16020.

Malsburg C von der (1973) Self-organization of orientation sensitive cells in the striate cortex. Kybernetik 100.

Matsuzaki M, Honkura N, Ellis-Davies GCR, Kasai H (2004) Structural basis of long-trm potentiation in single dendritic spines. Nature 429:761–766.

Miller K, MacKay D (1994) The role of constraints in Hebbian learning. Neural Comput.

Mitra A, Mitra SS, Tsien RW (2012) Heterogeneous reallocation of presynaptic efficacy in recurrent excitatory circuits adapting to inactivity. Nat Neurosci 15:250–257.

Murthy VN, Schikorski T, Stevens CF, Zhu Y (2001) Inactivity produces increases in neurotransmitter release and synapse size. Neuron 32:673–682.

Nahmani M, Turrigiano GG (2014) Deprivation-induced strengthening of presynaptic and postsynaptic inhibitory transmission in layer 4 of visual cortex during the critical period. J Neurosci 34:2571–2582.

Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG (2008) Strength through diversity. Neuron 60:477–482.

Nicholls JG (1956) The Electrical Properties of Denervated Skeletal Muscle. J Physiol 131:1–12.

Nick TA, Ribera AB (2000) Synaptic activity modulates presynaptic excitability. Nat Neurosci 3:142–149.

O’Brien RJ, Kamboj S, Ehlers MD, Rosen KR, Fischbach GD, Huganir RL (1998) Activity-dependent modulation of synaptic AMPA receptor accumulation. Neuron 21:1067–1078.

O’Leary T, van Rossum MCW, Wyllie DJA (2010) Homeostasis of intrinsic excitability in hippocampal neurones: dynamics and mechanism of the response to chronic depolarization. J Physiol 588:157–170.

O’Leary T, Williams AH, Franci A, Marder E (2014) Cell Types, Network Homeostasis, and Pathological Compensation from a Biologically Plausible Ion Channel Expression Model. Neuron 82:809–821.

Page 59: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

41

O’Leary T, Wyllie DJ a (2011) Neuronal homeostasis: time for a change? J Physiol 20:4811–4826.

Oja E (1982) A simplified neuron model as a principal component analyzer. J Math Biol 15:267–273.

Paoletti P, Bellone C, Zhou Q (2013) NMDA receptor subunit diversity: impact on receptor properties, synaptic plasticity and disease. Nat Rev Neurosci 14:383–400.

Pawlak V, Schupp BJ, Single FN, Seeburg PH, Köhr G (2005) Impaired synaptic scaling in mouse hippocampal neurones expressing NMDA receptors with reduced calcium permeability. J Physiol 562:771–783.

Penney J, Tsurudome K, Liao EH, Elazzouzi F, Livingstone M, Gonzalez M, Sonenberg N, Haghighi PA (2012) TOR Is Required for the Retrograde Regulation of Synaptic Homeostasis at the Drosophila Neuromuscular Junction. Neuron 74:166–178.

Petersen SA, Fetter RD, Noordermeer JN, Goodman CS, DiAntonio A (1997) Genetic analysis of glutamate receptors in drosophila reveals a retrograde signal regulating presynaptic transmitter release. Neuron 19:1237–1248.

Pouille F, Marin-Burgin A, Adesnik H, Atallah B V, Scanziani M (2009) Input normalization by global feedforward inhibition expands cortical dynamic range. Nat Neurosci 12:1577–1585.

Qiu Z, Sylwestrak EL, Lieberman DN, Zhang Y, Liu X-Y, Ghosh A (2012) The Rett syndrome protein MeCP2 regulates synaptic scaling. J Neurosci 32:989–994.

Rabinowitch I, Segev I (2006) The endurance and selectivity of spatial patterns of long-term potentiation/depression in dendrites under homeostatic synaptic plasticity. J Neurosci 26:13474–13484.

Ramakers G, Corner M, Habets A (1990) Development in the absence of spontaneous bioelectric activity results in increased stereotypical burst firing in cultures of dissociated cerebral cortex. Exp brain Res 141:537.

Ramiro-Cortés Y, Israely I (2013) Long lasting protein synthesis- and activity-dependent spine shrinkage and elimination after synaptic depression. PLoS One 8:e71155.

Renart A, Song P, Wang X-J (2003) Robust Spatial Working Memory through Homeostatic Synaptic Scaling in Heterogeneous Cortical Networks. Neuron 38:473–485.

Page 60: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

42

Rutherford LC, DeWan A, Lauer HM, Turrigiano GG (1997) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor mediates the activity-dependent regulation of inhibition in neocortical cultures. J Neurosci 17:4527–4535.

Rutherford LC, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG (1998) BDNF has opposite effects on the quantal amplitude of pyramidal neuron and interneuron excitatory synapses. Neuron 21:521–530.

Sandrock AW, Dryer SE, Rosen KM, Gozani SN, Kramer R, Theill LE, Fischbach GD (1997) Maintenance of acetylcholine receptor number by neuregulins at the neuromuscular junction in vivo. Science 276:599–603.

Sarti F, Schroeder J, Aoto J, Chen L (2012) Conditional RARα knockout mice reveal acute requirement for retinoic acid and RARα in homeostatic plasticity. Front Mol Neurosci 5:1–12.

Seeburg DP, Feliu-Mojer M, Gaiottino J, Pak DTS, Sheng M (2008) Critical Role of CDK5 and Polo-like Kinase 2 in Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity during Elevated Activity. Neuron 58:571–583.

Seeburg DP, Sheng M (2008) Activity-induced Polo-like kinase 2 is required for homeostatic plasticity of hippocampal neurons during epileptiform activity. J Neurosci 28:6583–6591.

Shepherd JD, Rumbaugh G, Wu J, Chowdhury S, Plath N, Kuhl D, Huganir RL, Worley PF (2006) Arc/Arg3.1 mediates homeostatic synaptic scaling of AMPA receptors. Neuron 52:475–484.

Shin SM, Zhang N, Hansen J, Gerges NZ, Pak DTS, Sheng M, Lee SH (2012) GKAP orchestrates activity-dependent postsynaptic protein remodeling and homeostatic scaling. Nat Neurosci 15:1655–1666.

Siddoway BA, Altimimi HF, Hou H, Petralia RS, Xu B, Stellwagen D, Xia H (2013) An essential role for inhibitor-2 regulation of protein phosphatase-1 in synaptic scaling. J Neurosci 33:11206–11211.

Siddoway BA, Hou H, Xia H (2014) Molecular mechanisms of homeostatic synaptic downscaling. Neuropharmacology 78:38–44.

Soden ME, Chen L (2010) Fragile X protein FMRP is required for homeostatic plasticity and regulation of synaptic strength by retinoic acid. J Neurosci 30:16910–16921.

Spiegel I, Mardinly AR, Gabel HW, Bazinet JE, Couch CH, Tzeng CP, Harmin DA, Greenberg ME (2014) Npas4 regulates excitatory-inhibitory balance within neural circuits through cell-type-specific gene programs. Cell 157:1216–

Page 61: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

43

1229.

Steinmetz CC, Turrigiano GG (2010) Tumor Necrosis Factor-a Signaling Maintains the Ability of Cortical Synapses to Express Synaptic Scaling. J Neurosci 30:14685–14690.

Stellwagen D, Malenka RC (2006) Synaptic scaling mediated by glial TNF-alpha. Nature 440:1054–1059.

Stroemer R, Kent T, Hulsebosch C (1995) Neocortical neural sprouting, synaptogenesis, and behavioral recovery after neocortical infarction in rats. Stroke 26:2135–2144.

Sutton MA, Ito HT, Cressy P, Kempf C, Woo JC, Schuman EM (2006) Miniature neurotransmission stabilizes synaptic function via tonic suppression of local dendritic protein synthesis. Cell 125:785–799.

Sutton MA, Taylor AM, Ito HT, Pham A, Schuman EM (2007) Postsynaptic Decoding of Neural Activity: eEF2 as a Biochemical Sensor Coupling Miniature Synaptic Transmission to Local Protein Synthesis. Neuron 55:648–661.

Sutton MA, Wall NR, Aakalu GN, Schuman EM (2004) Regulation of dendritic protein synthesis by miniature synaptic events. Science 304:1979–1983.

Tetzlaff C, Kolodziejski C, Timme M, Wörgötter F (2011) Synaptic scaling in combination with many generic plasticity mechanisms stabilizes circuit connectivity. Front Comput Neurosci 5:47.

Thalhammer A, Cingolani LA (2014) Cell adhesion and homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Neuropharmacology 78:23–30.

Thiagarajan TC, Lindskog M, Tsien RW (2005) Adaptation to synaptic inactivity in hippocampal neurons. Neuron 47:725–737.

Thiagarajan TC, Piedras-Renteria ES, Tsien RW (2002) α- and βCaMKII: Inverse Regulation by Neuronal Activity and Opposing Effects on Synaptic Strength. Neuron 36:1103–1114.

Thoby-Brisson M, Simmers J (1998) Neuromodulatory inputs maintain expression of a lobster motor pattern-generating network in a modulation-dependent state: evidence from long-term decentralization in vitro. J Neurosci 18:2212–2225.

Toyoizumi T, Kaneko M, Stryker MPP, Miller KDD (2014) Modeling the Dynamic Interaction of Hebbian and Homeostatic Plasticity. Neuron 84:497–510.

Page 62: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

44

Tripodi M, Evers JF, Mauss A, Bate M, Landgraf M (2008) Structural homeostasis: compensatory adjustments of dendritic arbor geometry in response to variations of synaptic input. PLoS Biol 6:e260.

Turrigiano G (2011) Too many cooks? Intrinsic and synaptic homeostatic mechanisms in cortical circuit refinement. Annu Rev Neurosci 34:89–103.

Turrigiano G, Abbott LF, Marder E (1994) Activity-dependent changes in the intrinsic properties of cultured neurons. Science 264:974–977.

Turrigiano G, Leslie KR, Desai NS, Rutherford LC, Nelson SB (1998) Activity-dependent scaling of quantal amplitude in neocortical neurons. Nature 391:892–896.

Wallace W, Bear MF (2004) A Morphological Correlate of Synaptic Scaling in Visual Cortex. J Neurosci 24:6928–6938.

Wang H-L, Zhang Z, Hintze M, Chen L (2011) Decrease in calcium concentration triggers neuronal retinoic acid synthesis during homeostatic synaptic plasticity. J Neurosci 31:17764–17771.

Wang T, Hauswirth AG, Tong A, Dickman DK, Davis GW (2014) Endostatin is a trans-synaptic signal for homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Neuron 83:616–629.

Wang X, Pinter MJ, Rich MM (2016) Reversible Recruitment of a Homeostatic Reserve Pool of Synaptic Vesicles Underlies Rapid Homeostatic Plasticity of Quantal Content. J Neurosci 36:828–836.

Wenner P (2011) Mechanisms of GABAergic homeostatic plasticity. Neural Plast 2011:489470.

Wierenga CJ, Ibata K, Turrigiano GG (2005) Postsynaptic expression of homeostatic plasticity at neocortical synapses. J Neurosci 25:2895–2905.

Wiesel TN, Hubel DH (1963) Single-Cell Responses in Striate Cortex of Kittens Deprived of Vision in One Eye. J Neurophysiol 26:1003–1017.

Xu X, Miller EC, Pozzo-Miller L (2014) Dendritic spine dysgenesis in Rett syndrome. Front Neuroanat 8:97.

Xue M, Atallah B V, Scanziani M (2014) Equalizing excitation-inhibition ratios across visual cortical neurons. Nature.

Yamahachi H, Marik SA, McManus JNJ, Denk W, Gilbert CD (2009) Rapid Axonal Sprouting and Pruning Accompany Functional Reorganization in Primary

Page 63: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

45

Visual Cortex. Neuron 64:719–729.

Yasumatsu N, Matsuzaki M, Miyazaki T, Noguchi J, Kasai H (2008) Principles of long-term dynamics of dendritic spines. J Neurosci 28:13592–13608.

Zenke F, Hennequin G, Gerstner W (2013) Synaptic Plasticity in Neural Networks Needs Homeostasis with a Fast Rate Detector. PLoS Comput Biol 9.

Zhang W, Linden DJ (2003) The other side of the engram: experience-driven changes in neuronal intrinsic excitability. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:885–900.

Zhao C, Dreosti E, Lagnado L (2011) Homeostatic synaptic plasticity through changes in presynaptic calcium influx. J Neurosci 31:7492–7496.

Zhong X, Li H, Chang Q (2012) MeCP2 Phosphorylation Is Required for Modulating Synaptic Scaling through mGluR5. J Neurosci 32:12841–12847.

Zuo Y, Lin A, Chang P, Gan W-B (2005) Development of long-term dendritic spine stability in diverse regions of cerebral cortex. Neuron 46:181–189.

Page 64: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

46

Page 65: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

47

Chapter 2

2 Structural correlates of Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity

Structural correlates of

Homeostatic Synaptic

Plasticity

Page 66: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

48

Contributions:

Anna F. Hobbiss and Inbal Israely designed experiments.

Anna F. Hobbiss performed experiments and analysed data.

Anna F. Hobbiss wrote the chapter.

Inbal Israely, Yazmín Ramiro Cortés, Ali Özgür Argunşah, Maria Royo and Inês Vaz

contributed with discussion about the work.

Page 67: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

49

2.1 Abstract

Homeostatic synaptic plasticity is known to change the strength of synapses in

response to chronic shifts in activity levels, through a process of multiplicative

synaptic scaling. It is not known whether structural parameters such as the

volume of a spine also scale simultaneously. Here we show that chronic activity

blockade using the Na2+ channel blocker TTX causes spines to increase in

volume, paralleling observed functional changes. At 48 hours, scaling is supra-

linear with an over-representation of large spines. By 72 hours scaling has re-

linearised. When activity blockade was removed, firing rate changes returned to

control levels by 96 hours, whereas structural changes required until 120 hours

to reverse the growth. Our data suggest that homeostatic synaptic plasticity

causes structural changes to spines in addition to functional changes. The circuit

is still functional after the expression of HSP and retains the ability to undergo

further homeostatic shifts if activity is again altered. In contrast, chronically

increased activity using GABAA blockers did not lead to synaptic down-scaling or

spine volume decreases in our system.

Page 68: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

50

2.2 Introduction

Homeostatic synaptic plasticity (HSP) causes global increases or decreases of

synaptic strength by changing the AMPA receptor content at synapses (O’Brien

et al., 1998; Turrigiano et al., 1998). This serves as a negative feedback

mechanism to counteract activity levels that are in a sub-optimal state, and bring

them back to within an ideal range. Synaptic strength changes have been shown

to increase in a linear multiplicative fashion, known as synaptic scaling, whereby

the relative synaptic strengths on a neuron are preserved. Although much has

been elucidated regarding the molecular pathways of HSP, very little is known

about how this affects the structure of the dendritic spine, the site of an

excitatory contact. Spines compartmentalise molecular processes and the

electrical coupling of the synapse to the dendrite, so changes to their volume and

morphology strongly modulate synaptic strength. Indeed, there is a very tight

structure-function relationship coupling the volume of the spine to its

conductance, with conductance increasing linearly with the volume of the spine

(Matsuzaki et al., 2001; Smith et al., 2003). Hebbian plasticity processes which

change the conductance of the spine also cause corresponding increases or

decreases in spine volume (Matsuzaki et al., 2004; Ramiro-Cortés and Israely,

2013). Whether similar structural processes exist in relation to HSP is an open

question under active investigation.

The current literature regarding spine size changes after chronic activity shifts is

equivocal. The first study into structural components of the synapse (Murthy et

al., 2001) showed that molecular components of both the pre-synaptic side, such

as the active area zone, and the post-synaptic side, i.e. the size of the post-

synaptic density, showed correlated increases after activity blockade. In

accordance with these results, two studies in vivo (Wallace and Bear, 2004; Keck

et al., 2013) also examined spine sizes in the visual cortex after sensory

deprivation, and found that the manipulation caused the average spine size to

Page 69: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

51

increase. This increase was reversible, with spines reducing to control levels

when animals were once more exposed to light. In contrast to this, various

studied in vitro have reported different structural dynamics after activity

changes. In organotypic slice cultures, spine fluctuations were measured over

several days of chemical activity manipulations (Yasumatsu et al., 2008). Here it

was observed that although activity blockade changes the dynamics of turnover

(with decreased turnover following blockade), it did not cause spine size changes

over days. Similarly, after a single-spine activity reduction protocol (Béïque et al.,

2011), no spine size difference was seen between silenced and control spines.

These contradictory results are surprising; it is not clear whether they are

attributable to different induction protocols or another experimental factor, or

whether the age or brain region studied can play a role. There is still clearly

considerable work to be done to reach a consensus on how HSP affects spine

sizes.

Another structural parameter which is subject to plasticity is spine density, and

this too is a source of contention in the HSP literature. Some studies have

reported a change in spine density following activity shifts; either an increase is

the number of spines following activity blockade (Stroemer et al., 1995; Burrone

et al., 2002; Kirov et al., 2004; Zuo et al., 2005b; Arendt et al., 2013) or a decrease

in spine density following activity decreases (Goold and Nicoll, 2010). Other

studies both in vivo and in culture have reported however that HSP induction

doesn’t cause a change in spine density (Wallace and Bear, 2004; Thiagarajan et

al., 2005). As with the spine size data described above, some of these results may

be explained by differences in HSP induction protocol, but exactly which

induction mechanisms lead to which spine density and size changes is still

subject to much debate.

Here we subject organotypic slice cultures to long-term activity manipulations of

up to 72 hours. We assess the induction of HSP using electrophysiological

Page 70: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

52

methods, and then examine the structural consequences of this induction. We

find that indeed HSP does have structural correlates, with 48 hours of activity

blockade using the Na2+ channel blocker TTX causing up-scaling of spine sizes in

whilst leaving the spine morphology distribution and spine density unchanged.

This increase in spine size is reversible, with removal of activity blockade leading

to decreases in spine volume over many days. Unlike the case for up-scaling,

chronic application of the GABAA blockers bicuculline and picrotoxin do not

cause downscaling either of functional synaptic strengths or of spine sizes.

Page 71: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

53

2.3 Materials and Methods

Throughout this thesis we use mouse organotypic hippocampal slice cultures to

study the structural effects of HSP at the level of individual synapses (Figure

2.1). Organotypic cultures allow for long-term chemical manipulations of

activity, which in vivo or acute slice methods could not support. The

developmental progress of neurons in slice cultures in terms of structure and

physiology remains very similar to that of neurons in vivo (De Simoni et al.,

2003), which makes this a good model to study the physiological processes of the

system. In contrast to primary neuronal cell cultures (in which most of the

previous HSP studies have been performed), the neuron structure and the

cellular composition of the system is preserved. This is especially important

when considering how a network-level effect, in this case altered activity levels,

will change the functioning of the system. Additionally, due to the laminar

organisation of the hippocampus, most of the connections between cells are

maintained in a slice, reducing the effects of rewiring (Gähwiler, 1997).

2.3.1 Slice culture preparation

Mouse organotypic slice cultures were prepared using p7-10 C57BL/6J mice.

Hippocampi were dissected and 350 µm slices were cut with a chopper in ice-

cold artificial cerebral spinal fluid (aCSF) containing 2.5 mM KCl, 26 mM NaHCO3,

1.15 mM NaH2PO4, 11 mM D-glucose, 24 mM sucrose, 1 mM CaCl2 and 5 mM

MgCl2. The slices were cultured on membranes (Millipore), and maintained at an

interface with the following media: 1x MEM (Invitrogen), 20% horse serum

(Invitrogen), 1 mM GlutaMAX (Invitrogen), 27 mM D-glucose, 30 mM HEPES, 6

mM NaHCO3, 2mM CaCl2, 2mM MgSO4, 1.2% ascorbic acid, 1 µg/ml insulin. The

pH was adjusted to 7.3, and osmolarity adjusted to 300–310 mOsm. All chemicals

were from Sigma unless otherwise indicated. Media was changed every 2-3 days.

Page 72: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

54

2.3.2 Biolistic Transfection

Pyramidal neurons from hippocampal organotypic slice cultures were

transfected using a Helios gene gun (Bio-Rad) after 4–7 days in vitro (DIV). Gold

beads (10 mg, 1.6 µm diameter, Bio-Rad) were coated with 100 mg AFP-plasmid

DNA (a GFP variant) according to the manufacturer’s protocol and delivered

biolistically to the slices, using a pressure of 160-180 psi.

Figure 2.1 Hippocampal organotypic slice cultures as a study system for Homeostatic

Synaptic Plasticity

a) Study system. Hippocampi from postnatal (p) 7-9 mice were dissected, sliced and cultured

for 7-9 days before the start of the experiment. All cells studied in this thesis were CA1

pyramidal cells. Imaging experiments were performed on secondary or tertiary apical

dendrites. b) Timeline for homeostatic synaptic plasticity induction. Slices were cultured at

p7-9 (days in vitro (DIV) 0). Sparse GFP labelling of pyramidal cells was achieved by biolistic

transfection at DIV 4-7. Chemical manipulations of activity were started at DIV 7-9

(designated 0 hours in experiments), and continued for up to 72 hours.

Page 73: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

55

2.3.3 Homeostatic plasticity induction by activity block

Tetrodotoxin (TTX) (1 µM, Tocris), Picrotoxin (100 µM, Abcam) or Bicuculline

(50 µM, Tocris) were added to the culture media at 7-9 DIV. The day of

application was then designated day 0 for experiments. Control experiments

were maintained in normal culture media, and were age-matched to treated

slices for experiments. For bicuculline and picrotoxin experiments an equivalent

volume of vehicle (DMSO) without the active compound was added to the control

culture medium. TTX was dissolved in H2O so no vehicle was necessary.

2.3.4 Patch clamp electrophysiology

Hippocampal slice cultures were perfused continuously with aCSF (as above,

with the addition of 0.5 µM TTX for all mEPSC recordings, and picrotoxin or

bicuculline respectively for experiments with these chemicals) for a pre-

incubation period of 15 to 30 min. Whole cell voltage-clamp recordings were

performed in CA1 pyramidal neurons, using 7–8 MΩ electrodes. For mEPSC

recordings, the internal solution contained 135 mM Cs-methanesulfonate, 10 mM

CsCl, 10 mM HEPES, 5 mM EGTA, 2 mM MgCl2, 4 mM Na-ATP and 0.1 mM Na-

GTP, with the pH adjusted to 7.2 with KOH, at 290-295 mOsm. Cells were voltage

clamped at -65 mV. Cellular recordings in which series resistance was higher

than 25 MΩ were discarded. Stability was assessed throughout the experiment,

with cells whose series resistance changed more than 30% being discarded.

mEPSCs recordings were started 3 minutes after break-in and continued for 10

minutes. Signals were acquired using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular

Devices), and data was digitized with a Digidata 1440 at 3 kHz. mEPSC events

were detected off-line using Mini-Analysis Program (Synaptosoft). Events

smaller than 15 pA were classified as indistinguishable from noise and were

discarded. For spontaneous activity recordings, slices were perfused

continuously with aCSF without the addition of TTX for a pre-incubation period

of 5 to 10 min. The internal solution for the electrodes contained 136.5 mM K-

Page 74: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

56

Glucagonate, 9 mM NaCl, 17.5 mM KCl, 10 mM HEPES, 0.2 mM EGTA, and 0.025

mM Alexa 594, with the pH adjusted to 7.2 with KOH, at 280-290 mOsm. In

current clamp with no external current applied, an IV curve was first recorded to

check for pyramidal-type firing patterns. Spiking events were then recorded for a

period of 6-9 minutes. The addition of Alexa-594 allowed cells to be imaged post-

recording to ensure they were pyramidal neurons. At the 48 hr timepoint,

activity was found to be highly correlated within slices (data not shown) so all

recordings from this timepoint are from cells in different slices. At 96 hours no

significant correlation was seen, so 1-3 cells were recorded per slice.

2.3.5 Two-photon imaging

Two-photon imaging was performed on a BX61WI Olympus microscope, using a

galvanometer-based scanning system (Prairie Technologies /Bruker) with a

Ti:sapphire laser (910 nm for imaging AFP; Coherent), controlled by PrairieView

software (Prairie Technologies). Slices were perfused with oxygenated aCSF

containing 127 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 25 mM NaHCO3, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 25 mM

D-glucose, 2 mM CaCl2 and 1 mM MgCl2 (equilibrated with O2 95%/CO2 5%) at

room temperature, at a rate of 1.5 ml/min. Activity blockade experiments

contained an additional 0.5 µM TTX in the aCSF for both experimental condition

and control; increased activity experiments contained either 50 µM bicuculline

or 100 µM picrotoxin in the aCSF. Secondary or tertiary apical dendrites of CA1

neurons (where the apical trunk is counted as the primary branch) were imaged

using a water immersion objective (60 x, 1.0 NA, Olympus LUMPlan FLN) with a

digital zoom of 8x. 2-3 dendrites were imaged per neuron. Z-stacks (0.5 µm per

section) were collected at a resolution of 1024 x 1024 pixels, resulting in a field

of view of 25.35 x 25.35 µm. 3 images were taken per dendrite at 5 minute

intervals, with the reported spine volume being the average of the 3 images.

Images were taken at the highest possible fluorescence value without leading to

Page 75: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

57

saturation of the image, to achieve maximum accuracy of volume quantification

(see below).

2.3.6 Spine volume determination

To determine the volume of spines we used SpineS, a custom built Matlab plug-in

which employs semi-automatic detection, automatic alignment and

segmentation of spine heads (Argunsah et al., 2016). Spine volume was

calculated using the summed fluorescence intensity of the spine, normalised to

the median fluorescence intensity of the dendrite, to correct for changes in

overall fluorescence levels in a cell. Fluorescence intensity was converted to real

volumes by taking Full Width Half Max (FWHM) measurements of spines

(Matsuzaki et al., 2004) and calculating a conversion factor between the two

measures. For the homeostatic plasticity analysis, all spines with a discernible

head within the field of view were included in the analysis, with the exception of

spines that were obstructed by other structures. This resulted in between 6 – 40

spines scored per dendrite.

2.3.7 Spine distribution and dendrite thickness

2-photon dendrite images were traced using Neurolucida software (MBF

Bioscience), adjusting tracing manually to follow the thickness of the dendrite.

Spine positions and spine type (mushroom, stubby, thin or filopodia) were

manually assigned.

2.3.8 Release of activity block

Slices were incubated for 48 hours in TTX as above. They were then removed

from TTX-containing media and moved to plates containing normal media. Media

was changed at least once more in the subsequent 2 days, to ensure complete

removal of TTX from the environment. Slices were imaged or recorded using

patch-clamp electrophysiology as described above.

Page 76: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

58

2.3.9 TSC1 knock-out mice generation

Tsc1fl/fl mice were crossed with Tsc1fl/+ Emx1Cre+/- mates. Pups with either a

Tsc1fl/fl Emx1Cre+ genotype (referred to as KOs (knock outs)) or a Tsc1fl/fl

Emx1Cre- genotype (referred to as controls) were used for cultures. Experiments

were performed blind to genotype.

2.3.10 Statistical analysis

All statistical analysis was done in Graphpad Prism 5. Stars represent the

degrees of significance, with 1 star (*) meaning p < 0.05; ** meaning p < 0.01; ***

meaning p < 0.001.

Page 77: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

59

2.4 Results

2.4.1 Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity induced by TTX-dependent

activity blockade

To chronically reduce activity levels in the slices, we used the Na+ channel

blocker Tetrodotoxin (TTX) to prevent action potentials, thus rendering the

slices completely silent. Although all activity is suppressed, other physiological

processes remain intact during TTX treatment. Neurons can survive for up to a

week until significant apoptosis occurs (Schonfeld-Dado and Segal, 2009), and

spine generation can still occur (Annis et al., 1994). Importantly, TTX does not

cause glial cell death even by 12 days, so any effects we see on neurons are not

due to changes in glial composition of the slices (Schonfeld-Dado and Segal,

2009). We therefore decided to examine the effects of TTX incubation for up to

72 hours, within the timeframe where there is no compromise of the health of

the slices.

2.4.1.1 HSP induced by TTX requires chronic blockade of activity

HSP has been reported to work over a wide range of timescales. Simple activity

blockade using TTX induces the expression of HSP after 1-2 days (Sutton et al.,

2006). Other protocols, which combine the silencing of action potentials and

blockade of NMDA receptors, induce HSP on much shorter timescales of around

2 hours (Sutton et al., 2004; Aoto et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2011). This form of fast

HSP is retinoic acid-dependent (Chen et al., 2014) (see part 1.5.1.2). For this

study, we decided to use only activity blockade via TTX, rather than the

induction protocol of activity blockade coupled with NMDA receptor blockade, in

order to mimic the physiological conditions in which HSP would occur such as

sensory deprivation. Our first step was therefore to verify that our activity

blockade protocol had no short-term consequences for the spines. We imaged

dendrites over 2 hours of TTX treatment, and compared spine volumes to that

Page 78: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

60

of dendrites imaged without any activity block. We saw no structural differences

between the neurons in TTX and those without it (Figure 2.2). Seeing as a linear

relationship between spine volume and conductance has been demonstrated

(Matsuzaki et al., 2001; Smith et al., 2003), the lack of changes over 2 hours

indicates that acute TTX treatment does not in itself induce structural or

functional plasticity.

2.4.1.2 Activity blockade over days causes functional synaptic scaling

We next sought to verify that as would be expected, activity blockade causes

functional HSP in our organotypic culture system. As a measure of functional

synaptic strength, we used whole cell patch clamp to record mini excitatory post-

Figure 2.2 Acute activity blockade does not cause structural changes

a) TTX application for 120 minutes did not lead to any changes in volume for the treated

spines. Imaging over 120 minutes also had no effect on spine volume, since the control also

showed no changes (p>0.05, ANOVA, post-hoc Bonferroni). n = 43 spines 3 cells control, 45

spines 3 cells TTX.

Page 79: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

61

synaptic potentials (mEPSCs) from CA1 pyramidal neurons. mEPSCs are the

result of action potential-independent fusion of a vesicle to a pre-synaptic

terminal, which releases glutamate into the synaptic cleft. This binds to

glutamate receptors on the post-synaptic terminal, resulting in channel opening

and current influx to the post-synaptic terminal. To ensure that no action

potential-dependent vesicle fusion occurred, we performed all experiments in

the presence of 0.5 µM TTX to block cell firing. The ‘control’ condition has thus

undergone acute TTX application for approximately 20 minutes (which does not

elicit plasticity, Figure 2.2) compared to the 48 hours treatment experienced in

the chronic ‘TTX’ condition. We clamped neurons at -65 mV to ensure that NMDA

receptors would be blocked and the currents we recorded were purely AMPA-

receptor mediated. The size of the mEPSC recorded at the soma can be

interpreted as proportional to the post-synaptic strength of the synapse, because

it will increase with increasing numbers of post-synaptic AMPA receptors. Since

spontaneous vesicle release happens stochastically, we can use the sizes of the

mEPSCs recorded over a period of time as a representative sampling of the

population of synaptic strengths of the neuron.

We recorded mEPSC amplitudes from neurons which had undergone 48 or 72

hours of TTX treatment, and compared them to age-matched controls (Figure

2.3a). We saw a significant increase in mEPSC amplitude in the TTX treated

condition by 48 hours, which was maintained at 72 hours (Figure 2.3b). This

demonstrates that synaptic strength was increased by TTX treatment in

accordance with the theory of HSP and experimental reports (Turrigiano et al.,

1998). We next checked whether the distribution of mEPSCs could be scaled

linearly as previously demonstrated (Turrigiano et al., 1998), to indicate that the

phenomenon of synaptic scaling had occurred. We saw that the distributions of

the treated events could indeed be scaled using linear equations, to overlay with

the control distributions (Figure 2.3 c). The fitting equations were: 48 hours Y =

X * 1.45 – 6.06, and 72 hours Y=X * 1.10 – 0.01.

Page 80: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

62

Figure 2.3 Activity block induces functional Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity

a) Example whole cell recording traces of mEPSCs from a control cell and a cell incubated in

TTX for 48 hours. Scale bar = 32 pA, 655 ms. Inset examples of average sized mEPSCs for the

48 hour timepoint. Scale bar = 20 pA, 30 ms. b) mEPSC amplitudes increase following TTX

treatment at both the 48 hour (p<0.001, n = 552 control, 742 TTX) and 72 hour (p<0.001,

Kruskal-Wallis, post-hoc Dunn, n= 1164 control, 1302 TTX) timepoints. c) Control amplitudes

can be scaled using first order equations to fit the recorded TTX amplitudes (48 hour scaling

shown in green: TTX = Control * 1.45 – 6.06. 72 hour scaling shown in blue: TTX = Control *

1.10 – 0.01). d) mEPSC intervals are altered differentially by TTX treatment at different

timepoint. Intervals are reduced at 48 hours (p<0.001, Kruskal-Wallis, post-hoc Dunn n = 544

control, 734 TTX ), but are increased by 72 hours (p<0.001 Kruskal-Wallis, post-hoc Dunn n =

1154 control, 1290 TTX).

Page 81: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

63

We then examined the frequency of mEPSCs; a proxy for pre-synaptic release

probability of the synaptic population. We saw different results between the two

timepoints we examined, with inter-mEPSC interval decreasing after treatment

at 48 hours, but increasing at 72 hours (Figure 2.3 d). Previous data about HSP

has been variable with regard to mEPSC frequency changes, (unlike the

consensus for mEPSC amplitude) with some studies reporting no change in

mEPSC frequency (O’Brien et al., 1998; Turrigiano et al., 1998; Kim and Tsien,

2008) whilst others find it altered (Thiagarajan et al., 2002, 2005). Although this

could be in part due to the system of study used, our data suggest that the length

of activity blockade could also explain these differences, with different cellular

mechanisms of compensation employed along a time-course of inactivity. The

studies in the literature are reported at various different timepoints, spanning

the range we examine here.

2.4.1.3 Functional synaptic scaling is accompanied by increases in spine

volume

After confirming the induction of functional HSP and synaptic scaling by

electrophysiology, we next investigated whether activity blockade had structural

consequences for spines. We used 2-photon microscopy to image apical (stratum

radiatum) dendrites of CA1 pyramidal neurons (Figure 2.4 a) which had been

pre-incubated for 0-72 hours in TTX (see Figure 2.1 for protocol). We measured

the volumes of all possible spines in the image using SpineS, a custom-written

Matlab plug-in, which uses summed fluorescence intensity to calculate volume

(Argunsah et al., 2016). Spines from TTX-incubated cells were significantly

bigger than those from control cells by 48 hours, and remained so by 72 hours

(Figure 2.4 b), showing that in accordance with the functional increases in

synaptic strength seen after TTX block, spines volumes do also increase in a

correlated manner.

We then investigated whether the distributions of volumes were linearly scaled

Page 82: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

64

(as had been the case for the functional HSP (Figure 2.3 c)). We saw that at 48

hours, volumes of TTX spines scaled supralinearly, being best fit by the function

Y = X2 * 5.44 + X * 0.25 + 0.055; in contrast, by 72 hours spine volumes scaled

linearly, being fit by Y = X * 1.26 – 0.004 (Figure 2.4 c). The supralinear scaling

at the 48 hours timepoint was due to a preponderance of large spines when

compared to controls. This result may indicate an interesting dissociation

Figure 2.4 Structural correlates of Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity

a) Representative example images of dendrites at either 0 hours or 48 hours after TTX

treatment or in control media. Asterisks indicate all the analysed spines for the respective

dendrite. Inset: zoomed images of individual spines at each time point. b) TTX treatment

causes an increase in spine volume by 48 hours of incubation (p<0.001, 2-way ANOVA,

post-hoc Bonferroni). This increase is maintained by 72 hours (p<0.05, 2-way ANOVA,

post-hoc Bonferroni). n of spines per timepoint = 103, 245, 285, 258 control; 172, 232,

208, 214 TTX. c) At 48 hours, TTX volumes show supralinear scaling with respect to

control (TTX = Control2 * 5.44 + Control * 0.25 + 0.55), whereas at 72 hours TTX volumes

scale linearly from controls (TTX = Control * 1.26 – 0.004)

Page 83: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

65

between the structure of the spine and the functional synaptic strengths, because

these large spines are not reflected in the mEPSC population data. By 72 hours

however the population structural distribution had reshaped to show linear

scaling between the control and the TTX population, suggesting that the

customary linear structure-function relationship (Matsuzaki et al., 2001; Smith et

al., 2003) has reasserted itself (Figure 2.4 c). It may be that since HSP is a very

slow process, the time taken for the normal processes which couple the volume

of the spine to its functional strength are also much longer, and so whilst the

functional expression of synaptic scaling has reached a stable distribution by 48

hours, the correlating structure takes an extra day to stabilise equally.

2.4.1.4 Distributions of types of spines after HSP is not altered

Up to this point in the thesis I have referred to structural changes only in the

context of the overall volume of spines. There are other parameters which can

change the signalling capabilities of spines, including head morphology

(Matsuzaki et al., 2001) and neck length and width (Tønnesen et al., 2014). In

particular, spines are traditionally characterised by being split into three

different groups; mushroom, stubby and thin (Harris et al., 1992), along with

non-mature protrusions called filopodia (Figure 2.5 a). Mushroom spines are

considered be the most mature spines – they have a well defined bulbous head

and a thin neck of varying lengths. Stubby spines also have a bulbous head but no

neck and appear to be continuous with the dendrite. Thin spines have no obvious

width difference between the neck and head and are considered less mature.

Finally, the least mature are the filopodia, extremely long, often bent protrusions

which are the precursors to mature synapses, and as such do not connect to a

pre-synaptic partner.

The proportions of these different types of spines change through development

(De Simoni et al., 2003), and shifts to the relative abundance of different spine

Page 84: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

66

types are also characteristic of various neurodevelopmental disorders such as

Fragile X syndrome (Irwin et al., 2000). We therefore investigated whether

inducing HSP changed the indistinguishable of the different types of spines. We

found that the proportions were identical between the control and the TTX

condition at the 48 hours timepoint (by which point structural changes to the

spine volume are already visible as reported in Figure 2.4). The proportions of

spines on each dendrite was extremely consistent, with mushroom being the

most populous (mean = 0.61 control, 0.64 TTX), followed by stubby (mean = 0.24

control, 0.23 TTX), thin (0.11 control, 0.08 TTX) and filopodia (0.03 control, 0.07

TTX) (Figure 2.5 b & c). We therefore conclude that although the volume of

spines changes after TTX treatment, the distributions of different spine

morphologies stays the same. This implies that the information content (believed

Figure 2.5 Distributions of spine morphologies are not affected by HSP at 48 hours

a) Spines were classified as either mushroom, stubby, thin or filopodia. Scale bar = 1µm. b)

Proportions of spines of different spine types were not affected by activity blockade. N of

dendrites = 17 control, 18 TTX. Lines represent mean values. c) Quantification of mean

proportions of spines of each type in the two different conditions

Page 85: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

67

to be stored in mature mushroom spines) has not been disrupted by the

induction of HSP, so the system has still maintained its function as a memory

storage and encoding structure. This idea of how the information coding capacity

of the system is altered following HSP will be explored further in Chapter 3.

2.4.1.5 Activity blockade does not change spine density or dendrite

volume

Structural plasticity can refer to not only the volume but also the number of

spines along a dendrite. Previous studies have shown conflicting results for

whether activity blockade causes changes to spine density; with some reporting

an increase in number of spines (Stroemer et al., 1995; Arendt et al., 2013) and

others showing no change (Thiagarajan et al., 2005). We manually traced

dendrites and spines using the Neurolucida programme to measure spine

density and also dendrite thickness, which is a physiological indicator of slice

and cell condition (Figure 2.6 a). We observed that the density of spines

increased over the time-course of our experiment (Figure 2.6 b). However, there

were no differences in spine density between the control and TTX conditions at

any timepoint, meaning that the overall increase is likely to be a normal process

occurring over the development of the neurons and the slice. In agreement with

our observations, spine turnover is maintained after activity block (Yasumatsu et

al., 2008), suggesting that the normal maturation processes are not disturbed by

blocking cell firing. Our data therefore agree with previous studies which do not

show an increase in spine density induced by homeostatic plasticity. To ensure

that activity blockade over 72 hours did not compromise cell health (Schonfeld-

Dado and Segal, 2009), we measured some structural parameters which could

reflect the viability of the cells. In experience we have observed that unhealthy

cells undergoing apoptosis display membrane blebbing (i.e. changes to

membrane thickness and beading along the dendritic branch). We therefore

measured dendrite thickness following chronic treatment with TTX as an

Page 86: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

68

indicator of neuronal health and found that this was constant throughout the

days of observation (Figure 2.6 c), showing that the TTX treatment was not

inducing visible dendritic damage.

Figure 2.6 Dendrite thickness and spine density are not affected by activity blockade

a) Dendrites and spines were manually traced in 3D using Neurolucida. Representative

images are from the 48 hour timepoint. Different colours of spines represent the different

classes described in section 2.4.1.4. Blue = mushroom; green = stubby; red = thin; yellow =

filopodia. b) Dendrite thickness remained constant over 72 hours of treatment and did not

differ between treated and control dendrites (p>0.05, ANOVA). c) Spine density increased

along the time-course of the experiment (p=0.01, ANOVA), but did not differ between

treated and control conditions at any timepoint (p>0.05, ANOVA). N of dendrites = 10, 9,

16, 19 control, 20, 9, 11, 21 TTX.

Page 87: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

69

2.4.2 Removal of activity block following HSP

For a system to be robust to changes in activity, it should maintain its ability to

re-adjust activity and synapse size following the induction of HSP. There are

some experimental indications that the changes wrought by HSP can be

reversed. HPS-dependent increased GluA1 receptor expression induced by AMPA

and NMDA receptor blockade starts to reverse by 24 hours out of inhibition, and

reaches control levels by 36 hours (O’Brien et al., 1998). However, this induction

protocol should induce the fast RA-dependent form of HSP (see section 1.5.1.2),

as opposed to our protocol which induces the slower non-RA dependent form. In

vivo, some of the structural and functional changes caused by dark rearing were

reversed after a brief re- exposure to light (Wallace and Bear, 2004). It is

remains unknown whether the structural changes induced by TTX blockade can

be reversed and, if so, the time-course over which this may happen. We therefore

subjected our slices to 48 hours of TTX treatment, and then removed slices from

the treated media and replaced them in normal, non-TTX containing media to

allow normal firing to recommence (Figure 2.7 a). We then examined the

functional and structural properties of the spines which had undergone removal,

as compared to control cells.

2.4.2.1 Firing rates return to control levels following removal of activity

block

In order to understand what kind of network activity occurs when activity

blockade is removed, we recorded firing rates of neurons at the 48 hours

timepoint (i.e. immediately after the removal of the TTX block) and at the 96

hour timepoint (48 hours after the removal of TTX) (Figure 2.7 b). It has been

reported that activity block causes an increase in firing rate once the blockade is

lifted (Turrigiano et al., 1998). To ensure that any firing rate effect we saw was

not due to a short-term rebound after lifting of the activity blockade, we added

an ‘acute TTX’ control to this experiment. For this condition, slices were kept in

Page 88: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

70

control conditions until the day of the experiment. At the beginning of the day

TTX was added to the media for 2-4 hours, to block all cell firing. At this point all

conditions (control, acute TTX and TTX) were treated equally. Slices were

removed from culture media and placed on the microscope whilst being

perfused with non-TTX containing aCSF. Activity could therefore take place

unchecked in all conditions. Firing rate was recorded between 20 - 300 minutes

Figure 2.7 Firing rates return to control levels after removal of activity block

a) Timeline for TTX removal experiments. Slices were prepared as in previous

experiments and incubated in TTX. After 48 hours, slices were removed from TTX-

containing media and placed in standard media. Controls were maintained in standard

media. b) Example traces of firing rates following 48 hours of activity block. Spikes are

demarked with black circles. Scale bar = 20 mV, 1 s. c) Quantification of firing rates. TTX

treated cells had a significantly higher firing rate than both controls at 48 hours (p<0.05),

directly after TTX removal. By 96 hours (i.e. 48 hours after TTX removal), firing rates had

become significantly smaller (p>0.01) and were indistinguishable from control levels. N =

17 control, 20 acute TTX, 25 TTX (48 hours); 15 control, 18 TTX (96 hours).

Page 89: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

71

after this. If activity block removal per say caused increased cell firing, it should

be observed in both the TTX and acute TTX condition.

We observed firstly that there was no significant difference between the control

and the acute TTX firing rates (Figure 2.7 b) (Mann-Whitney, p > 0.05). We

could therefore attribute any difference we saw between the control and TTX

conditions to the effects of HSP alone. Compared to the control condition, there

was an increased firing rate for the 48 hour TTX condition (Figure 2.7 b),

showing that as expected the synaptic and network changes wrought by HSP

cause an increase in activity. Interestingly, not only was the average firing rate

increased but the distribution of firing rates was also altered. In the control

condition, the vast majority of the cells had an extremely low firing rate

(between 0 and 8 spikes per minute) whilst a subset had much higher rates (up

to 100). This is in agreement with the accepted model of network firing rates in

the hippocampus and cortical circuits, where firing rate dynamics are extremely

skewed so that the majority of the spikes come from a small subset of cells

(Mizuseki and Buzsáki, 2013). However, in the TTX condition, there were no cells

with a near-zero firing rate. Instead, the majority of values clustered around the

median value of 13.3 spikes/min, with only one value which was much higher

than this. By the 96 hour timepoint, the TTX values had returned to control

levels, both in terms of the median firing rate and the distribution of the spikes

(Figure 2.7 b).

2.4.2.2 Spine volumes reverse after HSP-induced growth upon removal of

activity block

Having seen that firing rates recover to control levels after removal of activity

blockade, we next examined the spine volume to see if the same trend was

observed. We measured the spine volumes at 96 hours and 120 hours following

the initiation of the stimulation (Figure 2.8 a). We saw that the changes induced

through HSP at 48 hours had started to reverse by 96 hours; however the spines

Page 90: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

72

remain significantly bigger than control spines from the same developmental

time point (Figure 2.8 b). However, one day later at 120 hours post TTX

stimulation, the spines sizes have significantly decreased in size (Figure 2.8 b).

Closer examination of the distributions of spine sizes (rather than just the mean

and SEM) revealed that in fact the median at 120 hours post TTX was marginally

smaller than their control counterparts (p < 0.05) (Figure 2.8 c). Interestingly,

this finding supports findings from a previous in vivo study where prolonged

dark rearing caused spine head diameters to increase; but short re-exposure to

light caused sizes to shrink below the volumes of control animals which had

never experienced sensory deprivation (Wallace and Bear, 2004). We conclude

that the structural changes that we observe in response to HSP are indeed

reversible, although it takes up to 3 days after the removal of the activity block

before volumes are fully reversed. It is notable firing rates recover to control

levels by 96 hours (48 hours after removal of TTX), whilst spine structural

changes took until 120 hours (72 hours after the removal of TTX) to return to the

control sizes. This result, where the structural changes appear to ‘lag behind’ the

functional changes, echoes the data presented in Chapter 2, whereby linear

synaptic scaling was seen at 48 hours as measured functionally, but the linear

structural changes only manifested at 72 hours. Despite the slower time course

for structural changes to effects, it is clear that the circuit maintains its ability to

adapt to ongoing changes in overall activity after HSP, dynamically regulating its

synaptic strengths to optimise its function.

2.4.3 Tsc1 is not required for structural changes induced by HSP

Knowing that activity blockade causes structural changes to spines, we

wondered what signalling mechanisms could be involved in this phenomenon.

One avenue was suggested by the phenotype which we observed after activity

blockade – the emergence of large spines with bulbous heads (see Figure 2.4 for

example images). Interestingly, this same phenotype was observed in neurons

Page 91: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

73

which lack the gene encoding the hamartin protein, Tsc1, which is involved in

control of the mTOR signalling pathway (Tavazoie et al., 2005). Harmartin, along

with tuberin protein (encoded by the gene Tsc2), make up the mTOR

(mammalian Target of Rapamycin) suppressing Tuberous Sclerosis Complex

(TSC). We reasoned that since in a ‘normal’ state (i.e. without activity blockade),

Figure 2.8 Removal of activity block reverses HSP-dependent spine volume increase

a) Representative images from dendrites at 48 hour and 120 hour timepoints. Scale bar =

5µm. b) Spine sizes are increased by 48 hours of activity block as reported in section 0

(p<0.001). By 96 hours, spines have shrunk significantly (p<0.001) but are still larger than

controls. By 120 hours, spines have shrunk down to be even smaller than control spines

(p<0.05). Graph is plotted as mean + SEM. Stats = Kruskal-Wallis, post-hoc Dunn. n of

spines per timepoint = 285, 514, 326 control. 208, 430, 251 TTX. c) All spine sizes with

medians indicated with black bars.

Page 92: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

74

neurons lacking this gene phenocopy those displaying the structural effects of

HSP, the ability for the neurons themselves to further undergo structural scaling

may be impaired.

This idea was supported by the biology of Tsc1 function. The mTOR pathway

which Tsc1 modulates is an important signalling pathway involved in many

cellular processes, and which has multiple roles in neural functioning (Hoeffer

and Klann, 2010). One of the main functions of the mTOR pathway is to up-

regulate protein translation. In dendrites, this can occur when a strong plasticity-

inducing stimulus arrives at a synapse, activating the mTOR signalling pathway.

New protein synthesis occurs to generate the protein substrates for plasticity

processes, such as CaMKII and AMPARs. In baseline conditions, protein

translation is suppressed by TSC, which down-regulates mTOR signalling thus

stalling translation until the appropriate moment when the suppression is lifted.

Loss of either Tsc1 or Tsc2 makes the TSC complex dysfunctional, lifting the

brake on translation which leads to excessively high levels of proteins

(Kwiatkowski and Manning, 2005). In humans, mutations in Tsc1 or Tsc2 cause

the condition Tuberous Sclerosis, a disorder characterised by somatic and brain

tubers, mental retardation and a high incidence of epilepsy (Roach et al., 1998).

Relevantly for our study, loss of Tsc1 or Tsc2 leads to dendritic and spine

abnormalities, which have been suggested to be causal for the neuropsychiatric

phenotypes (Tavazoie et al., 2005; Zhou et al., 2006). The enlarged spine

phenotype previously mentioned could result from increased levels of protein in

the dendrite after loss of the TSC repressing effect, which allows for unchecked

plasticity. Loss of TSC suppression also causes pathologically high activity levels

in neural systems, likely for the same reasons. One characteristic feature of

Tuberous Sclerosis syndrome is epilepsy, which stems from hyperactivity and

overly correlated firing in neural systems (Roach et al., 1998). In studies on

cultured neurons, homozygous loss of Tsc1 caused the development of

hyperactive spontaneous activity levels (Bateup et al., 2013b), and induced

Page 93: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

75

neonatal seizures caused up-regulation of the mTOR pathway (Talos et al., 2012).

Combining both this structural and functional evidence, we hypothesised that

mechanisms of homeostatic synaptic plasticity could be impaired after loss of

TSC functioning, so the neurons are unable to regulate activity and spine sizes in

an appropriate way.

To test this hypothesis, we generated mice with homozygous knock-out (KO) of

Tsc1 in cortical neurons, by combining floxed Tsc1 with Cre-recombinase under

the control of an Emx1 promoter which expresses in cortical excitatory neurons

and glia (Gorski et al., 2002). Neurons were always compared to those from wild-

type litter-mate controls (i.e. double floxed Tsc1 but lacking the expression of

Cre-recombinase), which were age-matched to the treated cells, to account for

any effects from the genetic background of the mice. We then performed activity

blockade experiments using the same protocol as detailed before in Figure 2.1,

by incubating slices for 72 hours in TTX to block all activity and induce HSP. We

found that spine sizes showed a trend to be smaller at the 0 hours timepoint in

Tsc1- spines compared to wild-type, although the effect did not reach significance

(Figure 2.9). This is at odds with previous reports that spines are larger after

knock-out of the Tsc1 gene (Tavazoie et al., 2005). One possible explanation for

this is that the aforementioned study was carried out on isolated Tsc1- neurons

in a background of normal wild-type neurons, whereas our study constitutively

knocked out Tsc1 in all hippocampal neurons. In a wild-type background, a single

cell with increased firing rates may induce a large amount of LTP, rather than the

global homeostatic down-scaling response which should occur when in the

constitutive Tsc1- case when all cells have pathologically high firing. However,

counter to our initial hypothesis, the Tsc1- spines did show a HSP-induced

volume increase following 72 hours of TTX exposure, which mirrored the

increase seen in the wild-type cells after activity blockade (Figure 2.9).

Therefore, despite lacking the TSC suppression of translation, neurons were still

able to homeostatically scale upwards after activity blockade. This result accords

Page 94: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

76

with a study in primary cultured neurons which finds that Erk signalling but not

mTOR signalling were necessary for Arc-dependent synaptic scaling (Bateup et

al., 2013a). We therefore conclude that there is no evidence that removing the

brake on the mTOR pathway through loss of Tsc1 disrupts synaptic up-scaling in

these neurons.

Figure 2.9 Loss of Tsc1 does not impair structural HSP

Spines were imaged at 0 hours and at 72 hours. Tsc1- spines showed a trend to be

smaller than controls at timepoint 0, although this did not reach significance. By 72

hours, both Tsc1- and wild-type spines which had undergone TTX treatment were bigger

than control spines (ANOVA with post-hoc Bonferroni. n is shown as 0 hours and 72

hours. WT control = 138, 115. WT TTX = 149, 116. Tsc1- Control = 9, 182. Tsc1- TTX = 17,

60).

Page 95: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

77

2.4.4 GABAA antagonists fail to induce HSP in our system

Having seen structural and functional up-scaling after a chronic block of activity,

we next investigated if raising activity levels would induce a complementary

decrease of synaptic strength. One reported way to increase firing rates over

prolonged periods of time is to apply antagonists to GABAA receptors, which will

reduce inhibitory activity in the system and increase the excitatory drive onto

neurons. Due to this effect, GABAA blockade is used as a model for epilepsy

(Seeburg and Sheng, 2008). It has been reported that chronic application of

either one of the GABAA antagonists bicuculline or picrotoxin can cause

homeostatic decreases in firing rate and synaptic strength in primary neuronal

cell culture (Turrigiano et al., 1998). However, only one study has reported this

same effect in organotypic slices (Karmarkar and Buonomano, 2006); other

work in this model system has shown variable results, with mEPSC strengths

showing both decreases and increases depending on the timepoint at which they

were measured (Seeburg and Sheng, 2008). Since the anatomy and cellular

composition of monolayer primary cultures is extremely different to the more

physiologically conserved organotypic slices, it is quite possible that the systemic

response to manipulations could vary between the two models. We applied

bicuculline or picrotoxin to organotypic slices using the same timing protocol as

represented in Figure 2.1 b, for 0 – 72 hours. We did not observe either a

reduction in functional synaptic strength as measured by mEPSC amplitudes

(Figure 2.10 a & c), and as a likely consequence, nor in spine volume (Figure

2.10 b & d) following this manipulation. Instead, for both bicuculline and

picrotoxin mEPSC amplitudes were increased by the drug application at 48 hours

(although they had returned to control levels at 72 hours), mirroring the results

reported previously in organotypic slices (Seeburg and Sheng, 2008). We

conclude that in our organotypic slice preparation, the effect of decreasing

GABAA signalling is not enough to produce down-scaling of synaptic strengths.

Page 96: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

78

Figure 2.10 Chronic GABAA blockade does not induce downscaling through HSP

Chronic application of either of the GABAA antagonists Bicuculline (a,b) or Picrotoxin

(c,d) failed to induce homeostatic downscaling (i.e. reduced synaptic strength), as

measured either functionally by mEPSCs (a,c) or structurally by spine volume (b,d).

Bicuculline experiments: n of mEPSCs = 415, 971 control; 425, 912 bicuculline. n of

spines = 479, 847, 780, 690 control; 508, 704, 382, 618 Bicuculline. Picrotoxin

experiments: n of mEPSCs = 283, 412 contro; 480, 384 picrotoxin, n of spines = 81, 166,

149, 184 control; 111, 155, 217, 284 picrotoxin.

Page 97: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

79

2.5 Discussion

In this chapter, we showed that HSP induced by activity blockade causes

functional synaptic scaling in neurons (Figure 2.3), which is accompanied by a

corresponding increase in spine volume (Figure 2.4). The volumes show a

supra-linear scaling relationship by 48 hours, which returns to a linear scaling by

72 hours. Spine growth occurs without affecting other spine population

parameters such as distributions of morphology (Figure 2.5) or density (Figure

2.6). Both the structural and the functional changes are reversible, with firing

rates returning to control levels by 96 hours (or 48 hours out of activity

blockade, Figure 2.7), and spine volumes returning by 120 hours (Figure 2.8).

This structural scaling does not require the mTOR regulator Tsc1 (Figure 2.9).

2.5.1 Structural consequences of HSP

The elucidation of structural changes accompanying HSP adds a new facet to our

knowledge of this vital cellular process. Space within spines is extremely

restricted and so without physical adaptations such as increasing volume, there

would be very limited potential for a synapse to change its strength (Sala and

Segal, 2014). Some forms of synaptic adjustment can occur without removal or

insertion of receptors or other synaptic components – for example,

phosphorylation of surface receptors to maintain them in an open state for

longer – but these tend to be short-lived reversible states, and at the very longest

will not be maintained beyond the time it takes for that particular receptor to be

turned over. To produce adjustments with increased longevity, it is necessary to

introduce structural changes to spines, dendrites and cells.

It is therefore interesting that previous data is equivocal about the structural

consequences of homeostatic plasticity. Various studies, in a range of different

systems and using genetic or chemical protocols to block activity, have reported

that there is no consequent spine volume change (Yasumatsu et al., 2008; Béïque

Page 98: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

80

et al., 2011). It is uncertain why our results differ from these, although it is

possible to speculate on some of the reasons. In one study which characterises

how shifts in activity alter the dynamics of spine volume change over time, the

authors follow the same spines over time, and see no change (Yasumatsu et al.,

2008). Their protocol uses both TTX and NMDAR blockade which induces a

mechanistically different type of RA-dependent HSP. It is possible that this form

does not induce the structural changes seen after induction of the non-RA

dependent form in our results. The topic would benefit from further research to

resolve this question. A second study which reported no structural differences is

easier to reconcile with my results. In this work, activity is blocked by the over-

expression of the potassium channel Kir2.1 at upstream cells, and the

downstream spines are followed (Béïque et al., 2011). A lack of structural

changes could therefore be attributable to the fact that downstream cell firing,

which is itself sufficient to modulate synaptic scaling (Burrone et al., 2002; Ibata

et al., 2008), was not blocked, since silenced synapses made up only a small

proportion of the post-synaptic sites. Alternatively, their synaptic blockade may

not produce such strong results because it only lowers, rather than completely

abolishes, upstream firing and evoked synaptic activity.

Our data indicated that very large spines were over-represented after TTX

treatment (Figure 2.4). Although we do not know the original size of the spines

prior to TTX treatment, it is accepted that in general large spines are older and

more mature, whereas newly formed spines are small. Thus we can make an

educated guess that these large spines are the older ones in the culture, rather

than for instance being newly generated whilst in TTX blockade, and thus spines

which already exist on the branch are the first to show a HSP response, and grow

more. This may be because they already contain some of the components

necessary to engage the growth mechanisms, such as endoplasmic reticulum or

other organelles, whereas newly generated spines do not.

Page 99: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

81

2.5.2 Decoupling of structure and function

One interesting feature of the results presented in this chapter is the apparent

decoupling of the structure and function of dendritic spines after HSP. It is

generally accepted that under normal circumstances, there is a linear

relationship between a spine’s volume and its conductance (Matsuzaki et al.,

2001; Smith et al., 2003), which is maintained after Hebbian synaptic plasticity

(Govindarajan et al., 2011). Here we show that at the 48 hour timepoint of

activity blockade, mEPSC amplitudes scaled linearly between the TTX and

control conditions, as is commonly reported (Figure 2.3). However, at this same

timepoint spine volumes scaled supra-linearly compared to controls, requiring a

further 24 hours before the scaling of the volumes reflected that seen in mEPSC

amplitudes (Figure 2.4). Examination of the distributions showed a

preponderance of large spines (which is not reflected in the electrophysiological

data), indicating either that these spines grow first before the small spines catch

up by 72 hours, or that there is an over-growth of these spines, which then have

shrunk back down by 72 hours. Similarly, when the activity block was lifted to

allow action potentials to resume, firing rates returned to control levels by 96

hours (48 hours following lifting of activity block) but the structure took a

further 24 hours to shrink. It appears therefore that the structural changes

induced by HSP lag behind the functional changes. This mismatch between the

times of expression of the different processes could reflect the timeframe

required to co-opt different mechanisms for HSP. Functional changes, possibly

related to receptor insertion into pre-defined slots in the membrane, or different

receptor compositions (Lee et al., 2010), may be induced initially as a ‘first-pass’

attempt to equalise the functioning of the cells and circuits. If this doesn’t correct

the problem, structural changes may be implemented as a more extreme

measure. Interestingly, there is a previous suggestion that decoupling of

structure and function in spine head volume may be related to the proportion of

silent synapses in a population, which is increased after activity deprivation

Page 100: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

82

(Busetto et al., 2008; Ashby and Isaac, 2011). This topic will be explored in

greater detail in chapter 3 and in the general discussion in chapter 4.

2.5.3 Network changes after HSP

The changing of synaptic strengths may not only affect the activity of a single cell

but also the connectivity and shape of the network. Indeed, the data presented in

Figure 2.7 on firing rate changes throughout activity blockade and reversal

could represent an interesting shift in network dynamics after HSP. After 48

hours of activity blockade, the average firing rate was increased but the variance

of rates had decreased. In a standard cortical or hippocampal network, a

minority of cells will have very high firing rates, whilst the majority are near-

silent (Mizuseki and Buzsáki, 2013). After the TTX treatment however, there

were no cells with extremely low-firing rates, and most clustered around the

median value. Instead of the optimal coding strategy adopted by networks in a

normal state, activity block seems to homogenise firing rates to bring all the cells

up to approximately similar values. This may sensitise the system as a whole to

any inputs that arrive by giving all cells similar weights in the network dynamics,

as opposed to the highly skewed distribution seen in normal networks. In such

cases, if after a period of inactivity new activity is applied (for instance, if after a

period of sensory deprivation, stimuli again arise), the network can relearn an

optimal state for the new type of activity it receives, reshaping itself over days to

form the normal network state of highly skewed firing rates, appropriate for the

new patterns of activity.

2.5.4 Tsc1 signalling and HSP

Some indication that synaptic down-scaling could be impaired is seen in the

necessity for mTOR control to regulate a rodent model for early-life seizures

(Talos et al., 2012). Our data however showed no requirement for the Tsc1 gene

for the expression of structural up-scaling (Figure 2.9). This data is preliminary

Page 101: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

83

since the sample size is small. It would be interesting to know whether the loss of

Tsc1 affects down-scaling (instead of up-scaling), since it is the inability to

regulate activity downwards which characterises these systems, leading them to

exhibit pathologically hyperactive firing rates. It may be that down-scaling is in

fact impaired. On the other hand, it is possible that the decreased average spine

size we observed at the 0 hours timepoint is the result of maximal synaptic

downscaling, in an unsuccessful attempt to return activity levels to normal. This

would imply that structural scaling is intact but is not effective enough to

compensate for the loss of mTOR control. Since we were unable to reliably

induce down-scaling in our system (Figure 2.10) we did not pursue these

experiments. Whether or not Tsc1- neurons can bi-directionally regulate activity

levels therefore remains an interesting open question in the field. If not, loss of

homeostatic control could account for the mental retardation, epilepsy and spine

abnormality phenotypes seen in mouse models and human patients with this

disorder.

Page 102: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

84

2.6 Bibliography

Annis CM, Odowd DK, Robertson RT (1994) Activity-Dependent Regulation of Dendritic Spine Density on Cortical Pyramidal Neurons in Organotypic Slice Cultures. J Neurobiol 25:1483–1493.

Aoto J, Nam CI, Poon MM, Ting P, Chen L (2008) Synaptic Signaling by All-Trans Retinoic Acid in Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity. Neuron 60:308–320.

Arendt KL, Sarti F, Chen L (2013) Chronic inactivation of a neural circuit enhances LTP by inducing silent synapse formation. J Neurosci 33:2087–2096.

Argunsah AO, Erdil E, Ghani M., Yagci AM, Ramiro-Cortes Y, Hobbiss AF, Kanik S., Cetin M, Israely I, Unay D (2016) SpineS: A Tool for Automatic Tracking of Dendritic Spines. “In prep.”

Ashby MC, Isaac JTR (2011) Maturation of a Recurrent Excitatory Neocortical Circuit by Experience-Dependent Unsilencing of Newly Formed Dendritic Spines. Neuron 70:510–521.

Bateup HS, Denefrio CL, Johnson CA, Saulnier JL, Sabatini BL (2013a) Temporal dynamics of a homeostatic pathway controlling neural network activity. Front Mol Neurosci 6:28.

Bateup HS, Johnson CA, Denefrio CL, Saulnier JL, Kornacker K, Sabatini BL (2013b) Excitatory/inhibitory synaptic imbalance leads to hippocampal hyperexcitability in mouse models of tuberous sclerosis. Neuron 78:510–522.

Béïque JJ-C, Na Y, Kuhl D, Worley PF, Huganir RL (2011) Arc-dependent synapse-specific homeostatic plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108:816–821.

Burrone J, O’Byrne M, Murthy VN (2002) Multiple forms of synaptic plasticity triggered by selective suppression of activity in individual neurons. Nature 420.

Busetto G, Higley MJ, Sabatini BL (2008) Developmental presence and disappearance of postsynaptically silent synapses on dendritic spines of rat layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons. J Physiol 586:1519–1527.

Chen L, Lau AG, Sarti F (2014) Synaptic retinoic acid signaling and homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Neuropharmacology 78:3–12.

De Simoni A, Griesinger CB, Edwards FA (2003) Development of rat CA1 neurones in acute versus organotypic slices: role of experience in synaptic

Page 103: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

85

morphology and activity. J Physiol 550:135–147.

Gähwiler B (1997) Organotypic slice cultures: a technique has come of age. Trends Neurosci 20:471–477.

Goold CP, Nicoll RA (2010) Single-cell optogenetic excitation drives homeostatic synaptic depression. Neuron 68:512–528.

Gorski JA, Talley T, Qiu M, Puelles L, Rubenstein JLR, Jones KR (2002) Cortical excitatory neurons and glia, but not GABAergic neurons, are produced in the Emx1-expressing lineage. J Neurosci 22:6309–6314.

Govindarajan A, Israely I, Huang S-Y, Tonegawa S (2011) The dendritic branch is the preferred integrative unit for protein synthesis-dependent LTP. Neuron 69:132–146.

Harris KM, Jensen FE, Tsao B (1992) Three-Dimensional Structure of Dendritic Spines and Synapses in Rat Hippocampus (CA1) at Postnatal Day 15 and Adult Ages : Implications for the Maturation of Synaptic Physiology and Long-term Potentiation. J Neurosci 12.

Hoeffer CA, Klann E (2010) mTOR signaling: At the crossroads of plasticity, memory and disease. Trends Neurosci 33:67–75.

Ibata K, Sun Q, Turrigiano GG (2008) Rapid Synaptic Scaling Induced by Changes in Postsynaptic Firing. Neuron 57:819–826.

Irwin SA, Galvez R, Greenough WT (2000) Dendritic spine structural anomalies in fragile-X mental retardation syndrome. Cereb Cortex 10:1038–1044.

Karmarkar UR, Buonomano D V (2006) Different forms of homeostatic plasticity are engaged with distinct temporal profiles. Eur J Neurosci 23:1575–1584.

Keck T, Keller GB, Jacobsen RI, Eysel UT, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M (2013) Synaptic scaling and homeostatic plasticity in the mouse visual cortex in vivo. Neuron 80:327–334.

Kim J, Tsien RW (2008) Synapse-specific adaptations to inactivity in hippocampal circuits achieve homeostatic gain control while dampening network reverberation. Neuron 58:925–937.

Kirov SA, Goddard CA, Harris KM (2004) Age-dependence in the homeostatic upregulation of hippocampal dendritic spine number during blocked synaptic transmission. Neuropharmacology 47:640–648.

Kwiatkowski DJ, Manning BD (2005) Tuberous sclerosis: A GAP at the

Page 104: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

86

crossroads of multiple signaling pathways. Hum Mol Genet 14.

Lee M-C, Yasuda R, Ehlers MD (2010) Metaplasticity at Single Glutamatergic Synapses. Neuron 66:859–870.

Matsuzaki M, Ellis-Davies GC, Nemoto T, Miyashita Y, Iino M, Kasai H (2001) Dendritic spine geometry is critical for AMPA receptor expression in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Nat Neurosci 4:1086–1092.

Matsuzaki M, Honkura N, Ellis-Davies GCR, Kasai H (2004) Structural basis of long-trm potentiation in single dendritic spines. Nature 429:761–766.

Mizuseki K, Buzsáki G (2013) Preconfigured, skewed distribution of firing rates in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex. Cell Rep 4:1010–1021.

Murthy VN, Schikorski T, Stevens CF, Zhu Y (2001) Inactivity produces increases in neurotransmitter release and synapse size. Neuron 32:673–682.

O’Brien RJ, Kamboj S, Ehlers MD, Rosen KR, Fischbach GD, Huganir RL (1998) Activity-dependent modulation of synaptic AMPA receptor accumulation. Neuron 21:1067–1078.

Ramiro-Cortés Y, Israely I (2013) Long lasting protein synthesis- and activity-dependent spine shrinkage and elimination after synaptic depression. PLoS One 8:e71155.

Roach ES, Gomez MR, Northrup H (1998) Tuberous Sclerosis Complex Consensus Conference: Revised Clinical Diagnostic Criteria. J Child Neurol 13:624–628.

Sala C, Segal M (2014) Dendritic Spines: The Locus of Structural and Functional Plasticity. Physiol Rev 94:141–188.

Schonfeld-Dado E, Segal M (2009) Activity-dependent survival of neurons in culture: a model of slow neurodegeneration. J Neural Transm 116:1363–1369.

Seeburg DP, Sheng M (2008) Activity-induced Polo-like kinase 2 is required for homeostatic plasticity of hippocampal neurons during epileptiform activity. J Neurosci 28:6583–6591.

Smith MA, Ellis-Davies GCR, Magee JC (2003) Mechanism of the distance-dependent scaling of Schaffer collateral synapses in rat CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Physiol 548:245–258.

Stroemer R, Kent T, Hulsebosch C (1995) Neocortical neural sprouting,

Page 105: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

87

synaptogenesis, and behavioral recovery after neocortical infarction in rats. Stroke 26:2135–2144.

Sutton MA, Ito HT, Cressy P, Kempf C, Woo JC, Schuman EM (2006) Miniature neurotransmission stabilizes synaptic function via tonic suppression of local dendritic protein synthesis. Cell 125:785–799.

Sutton MA, Wall NR, Aakalu GN, Schuman EM (2004) Regulation of dendritic protein synthesis by miniature synaptic events. Science 304:1979–1983.

Talos DM, Sun H, Zhou X, Fitzgerald EC, Jackson MC, Klein PM, Lan VJ, Joseph A, Jensen FE (2012) The interaction between early life epilepsy and autistic-like behavioral consequences: A role for the mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR) pathway. PLoS One 7:1–16.

Tavazoie SF, Alvarez VA, Ridenour DA, Kwiatkowski DJ, Sabatini BL (2005) Regulation of neuronal morphology and function by the tumor suppressors Tsc1 and Tsc2. Nat Neurosci 8:1727–1734.

Thiagarajan TC, Lindskog M, Tsien RW (2005) Adaptation to synaptic inactivity in hippocampal neurons. Neuron 47:725–737.

Thiagarajan TC, Piedras-Renteria ES, Tsien RW (2002) α- and βCaMKII: Inverse Regulation by Neuronal Activity and Opposing Effects on Synaptic Strength. Neuron 36:1103–1114.

Tønnesen J, Katona G, Rózsa B, Nägerl UV (2014) Spine neck plasticity regulates compartmentalization of synapses. Nat Neurosci 17:678–685.

Turrigiano G, Leslie KR, Desai NS, Rutherford LC, Nelson SB (1998) Activity-dependent scaling of quantal amplitude in neocortical neurons. Nature 391:892–896.

Wallace W, Bear MF (2004) A Morphological Correlate of Synaptic Scaling in Visual Cortex. J Neurosci 24:6928–6938.

Wang H-L, Zhang Z, Hintze M, Chen L (2011) Decrease in calcium concentration triggers neuronal retinoic acid synthesis during homeostatic synaptic plasticity. J Neurosci 31:17764–17771.

Yasumatsu N, Matsuzaki M, Miyazaki T, Noguchi J, Kasai H (2008) Principles of long-term dynamics of dendritic spines. J Neurosci 28:13592–13608.

Zhou X, Zhu J, Liu K, Sabatini BL (2006) Mutual Information-Based Feature Selection in Studying Perturbation of Dendritic Structure Caused by TSC2 Inactivation. 00:81–94.

Page 106: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

88

Zuo Y, Yang G, Kwon E, Gan W-B (2005) Long-term sensory deprivation prevents dendritic spine loss in primary somatosensory cortex. Nature 436:261–265.

Page 107: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

89

Chapter 3

3 Threshold modulation of LTP induction by Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity

Threshold modulation of LTP

induction by Homeostatic

Synaptic Plasticity

Page 108: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

90

Contributions:

Anna F. Hobbiss and Inbal Israely designed experiments.

Anna F. Hobbiss performed experiments and analysed data. Ali Özgür Argunşah

provided Matlab code for analysis in Figure 3.3

Anna F. Hobbiss wrote the chapter.

Inbal Israely, Yazmín Ramiro Cortés, Ali Özgür Argunşah, Maria Royo and Inês Vaz

contributed with discussion about the work.

Page 109: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

91

3.1 Abstract

Homeostatic and Hebbian plasticity represent opposing forces for the strength of

a synapse. How these two types of plasticity interact, and which parts of the cell

or circuit are most susceptible to each, is still to be understood. Here we use TTX

to induce HSP through global block activity, and subsequently induce LTP at

single spines using glutamate uncaging. We find that LTP in single spines is

maintained after HSP, and has an increased longevity compared to LTP in control

spines. The induction of LTP is more efficacious after HSP, especially in small

spines, and can be induced by lower levels of glutamate. Neighbouring spines

also show slight growth after HSP. This led us to propose that the threshold to

induce LTP is reduced by HSP expression, sensitising the circuit to any new

activity it receives.

Page 110: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

92

3.2 Introduction

Homeostatic and Hebbian plasticity are utilised by neural systems for different

purposes, namely stability and information coding respectively. There is strong

evidence that both rely on calcium signalling to sense the activity changes of the

post synaptic cell (Sabatini et al., 2001; Pawlak et al., 2005; Ibata et al., 2008).

Hebbian plasticity will strengthen a synapse when post-synaptic activity is high,

and weaken it when activity is low. Homeostatic synaptic plasticity applies the

opposite logic, decreasing synaptic strength when post-synaptic activity is high

and strengthening it when it drops. Although these two types of plasticity have

opposing modes of expression, in vivo they will co-exist temporally and spatially.

How they interact is an open question in the field of synaptic plasticity.

Two different possibilities can be postulated for how HSP affects the expression

of Hebbian LTP. One theory is that since HSP has already strengthened the

synapses by enlarging them and increasing their AMPAR content, they will reach

a ceiling for plasticity and LTP will be occluded. Alternatively, since the goal of

HSP is increase input to a cell, it could act to sensitise the synapse, so allowing it

to express more LTP. Which of these two scenarios occurs is an empirically

addressable question. Some recent studies have sought to understand the effect

of HSP on LTP expression, using various different induction methods for the two

different types of plasticity (Lee et al., 2010; Arendt et al., 2013; Félix-Oliveira et

al., 2014). One approach was to induce both HSP and LTP at single synapses,

using genetic methods and glutamate uncaging respectively (Lee et al., 2010).

This allows for comparisons of the affected spines with non-affected neighbours

in the same slice. At silenced synapses NMDAR currents were enhanced and

structural and functional LTP was increased. It is not clear whether this local

method of synaptic silencing is functionally equivalent to global HSP; and indeed

the authors of this study refer to the phenomenon as ‘metaplasticity’ rather than

homeostasis. Alternatively, both HSP and LTP can be induced on a wider scale

Page 111: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

93

(Arendt et al., 2013). In this study, the authors globally induced HSP using TTX

application, and then used electrical stimulation to induce LTP at multiple

synapses. They observed enhanced LTP and an increased proportion of newly

formed silent synapses after activity blockade. Silent synapses lack AMPA

receptors, and so do not participate in an excitatory post-synaptic current (EPSC)

response. The electrical LTP stimulus activates NMDA receptors due to the co-

incidence of glutamate binding and post-synaptic depolarisation from a back

propagating action potential. This allows calcium influx to the spine leading to

AMPA receptor insertion, so unsilencing the synapse. It will now functionally

contribute to post-synaptic EPSCs, so the response to stimulation will be higher.

The relative magnitude of LTP will be greatly enhanced by an increased number

of silent synapses, because the potential for change is larger. An increase in

newly formed silent synapses is also supported by previous data on AMPA

insertion following activity blockade (Thiagarajan et al., 2005), where GluA1 is

inserted into pre-existing synapses but not new ones, leaving the newly formed

synapses without AMPARs and therefore silent. This mechanism will cause

‘priming’ of a circuit to increase the magnitude of LTP in response to many

stimuli arriving simultaneously – as indeed happens during early developmental

timepoints when silent synapses are common and contribute to circuit

refinement (Busetto et al., 2008). Normally however activity does not arrive

simultaneously at multiple synapses, but at individual ones. There are important

mechanistic differences between stimulating a single spine and multiple spines,

because synapses compete for the use of resources such as proteins when

stimulated in close succession (Fonseca et al., 2004; Govindarajan et al., 2011).

This affects the outcome of plasticity, where competing spines could achieve a

lower level of LTP or enter a ‘win-or-lose’ contest for plasticity, with winning

synapses expressing normal LTP but losing synapses none. It is unknown what

the response of a synapse stimulated individually will be after HSP, especially if

that synapse is initially silent.

Page 112: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

94

There is still much to be discovered about how HSP modulates the expression of

Hebbian plasticity, especially in the most physiologically realistic cases of a

globally induced HSP and a locally induced Hebbian plasticity. In this chapter, we

use single-spine induction of LTP on a background of homeostatic plasticity to

address whether there is a constructive or competitive interaction between the

two plasticity types. We find that after HSP, LTP at single spines is longer lasting,

and is enhanced in small spines as compared to large spines. Spines also respond

to lower levels of glutamate following HSP. Neighbouring spines close to the

stimulated spine show a short-lasting structural potentiation. This leads us to

propose that HSP decreases the threshold for LTP expression.

Page 113: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

95

3.3 Materials and Methods

3.3.1 Sample preparation

Slice cultures, transfection and homeostatic plasticity induction using TTX were

performed as described in the previous chapter. Unless otherwise specified, all

experiments in this chapter are from the time-point of 48 hours of TTX

application, or from age matched controls which have not undergone activity

block.

3.3.2 Two-Photon imaging

Imaging experiments were performed using the same set-up and methods as

described in the previous chapter. For uncaging experiments, imaging was

performed at 10x magnification, giving a field of view of 20.28 x 20.28 µm.

Imaged dendrites contained both stimulated spines (see below) and

neighbouring spines. Neighbouring spines could be anywhere in the field of view,

i.e. up to a maximum distance of ~30 µm on the dendrite. 2 – 10 neighbours were

measured per experiment.

3.3.3 Glutamate Uncaging

MNI-caged-L-glutamate (Tocris) was dissolved in aCSF lacking MgCl2 or CaCl2 in

the dark to make a stock concentration of 10 mM. Individual aliquots were

diluted to the working concentration of 2.5 mM in uncaging aCSF (see below), in

3 ml volumes. To test each individual stock of MNI-glutamate, whole cell patch

clamp was performed as described in the previous chapter and uncaging was

applied to single spines. Uncaging-evoked EPSCs were measured at the soma. For

imaging experiments, slices were incubated for 30-45 minutes in normal aCSF

(Materials and Methods, Chapter 2) containing 0.5 µM TTX. Imaging was

performed using the same system as in Chapter 2. Uncaging was performed

using a second Ti:sapphire laser (720 nm to uncage MNI-glutamate; Coherent),

Page 114: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

96

controlled by PrairieView software (Prairie Technologies, Bruker). All uncaging

stimuli were performed in the presence of uncaging-aCSF lacking MgCl2 and

containing 4 mM CaCl2. The uncaging train consisted of 30 pulses at 0.5 Hz, with

a pulse width of either 4 ms (supra-threshold) or 1 ms (sub-threshold), using

30mW power as measured at the back aperture. The uncaging point was

positioned 0.6 µm from the end of the spine head. To control for non-glutamate

dependent laser effects, ‘sham’ stimulations (Figure 3.10) were performed after

5 minutes of perfusion with the uncaging-aCSF without the addition of MNI-

glutamate. If no spine growth was seen, the perfusion was switched to

recirculation of uncaging-aCSF containing 2.5mM MNI-glutamate, and the ‘real’

stimulus was applied after 5 minutes. Fast imaging (approximately 20 Hz) of a

region of interest (ROI) of the stimulated spine head and neck was performed

throughout the 60 second stimulation, to record the growth dynamics for this

time period. The perfusion was then immediately switched back to the normal

aCSF for the remainder of the experiment (1-2 hours). The first image was taken

immediately, and is designated 0 minute post stimulation. In practice, due to the

time taken to record the imaging stack, this image will be approximately 1

minute after the end of the 60s stimulation.

3.3.4 Spine volume determination

Volume of spines was determined using the Matlab plug-in Spines as described in

the Chapter 2 (Argunsah et al., 2016).

3.3.5 Statistical analysis

All statistical analysis was done in Graphpad Prism 5. Stars represent the

degrees of significance, with 1 star (*) meaning p < 0.05; ** meaning p < 0.01; ***

meaning p < 0.001.

Page 115: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

97

3.4 Results

3.4.1 LTP following induction of HSP

In vivo, homeostatic normalisation of synaptic strengths must occur concurrently

with synapse-specific, information-encoding Hebbian plasticity. The interplay

between these two types of plasticity is yet to be understood. Previous

experiments exploring how global HSP affects the expression of LTP have used

induction protocols which stimulate multiple axon fibres, resulting in a multi-

spine LTP (Arendt et al., 2013; Félix-Oliveira et al., 2014). Realistically however,

activity will arrive at single inputs on a dendrite rather than many

simultaneously. This can have important consequences for the output of

plasticity, since synapses compete for the use of resources such as proteins when

stimulated at the same time (Fonseca et al., 2004; Govindarajan et al., 2011). We

therefore opted to induce LTP only at single inputs, to avoid the confound of

synaptic competition and understand more precisely how the global

phenomenon of HSP and the local phenomenon of LTP co-exist and interact.

3.4.1.1 Single spine LTP induction protocol

All the following experiments were performed after 48 hours of incubation in

TTX (Figure 3.1 a). We used glutamate uncaging (Figure 3.1 b - e) to stimulate

individual synapses (Pettit et al., 1997). The uncaging technique involves using

light to photolyse a molecule of caged MNI-glutamate – a glutamate molecule

bound to an MNI ‘cage’, which inactivates it – releasing the active glutamate

(Figure 3.1 b) (Palma-Cerda et al., 2012). In this study we use a 2-photon laser

to photolyse the glutamate, thus taking advantage of one of the great advantages

of 2-photon microscopy – its extremely small point of excitation (Denk et al.,

1990). Release of glutamate is localised to individual spines, allowing for the

induction of LTP at only one spine on a dendrite, whilst neighbouring spines

remain unaffected (Figure 3.1 d).

Page 116: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

98

The uncaging protocol used was based on previously described stimulation

protocols to induce early LTP (E-LTP), and consisted of 1 minute of stimulation

pulses at 0.5 Hz, with a stimulation strength of 30mW and a pulse width of 4 ms

Figure 3.1 Protocol for uncaging experiments

a) Timeline for uncaging experiments. Slices were cultured at p7-9 (DIV 0) and transfected

at DIV 4-7. Chemical manipulations of activity were started at DIV 7-9 (designated 0

hours). Uncaging was performed at the 48 hour time-point. b) MNI-Caged glutamate

photolysis. Glutamate is inactivated by a bound MNI ‘cage’. 720 nm 2-photon laser

excitation cleaves the MNI-group, releasing the active glutamate. c) Uncaging-induced

structural plasticity. Stimulation of a single spine (yellow arrowhead and inset yellow box)

causes localised spine growth, whilst neighbouring spines (blue arrowhead and inset blue

box) remain unchanged. d) Example of volume changes over an E-LTP experiment.

Page 117: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

99

(Harvey and Svoboda, 2007; Govindarajan et al., 2011). The stimulations were

performed in aCSF lacking Mg2+, to allow NMDA receptor activation. This

protocol has been shown to produce structural LTP lasting approximately 1

hour, but which will decay to baseline thereafter, known as early LTP or E-LTP

(Figure 3.1 d). Other stimulation protocols have also been developed which

produce longer-lasting (late) potentiation (L-LTP) (Govindarajan et al., 2011),

which combine the same uncaging stimulus with chemical activation of cAMP

activity. We chose to induce E-LTP rather than L-LTP in this study because then

rather than being at a ceiling for plasticity induction we would be in the middle

of a dynamic range, allowing us to detect any changes in magnitude which

occurred either upwards or downwards after HSP. To control for any physical

effects of laser stimulation (independent of glutamate) on the spine, we

performed a ‘sham’ uncaging stimulus using exactly the same stimulation

protocol but without the presence of MNI-glutamate on spines from a different

branch than the one selected for the ‘real’ stimulation (with MNI-glutamate), at

the beginning of the experiment. If any growth was detected in the spines after

the sham stimulation, the experiment was discarded. If there was no change in

the stimulated spines, the perfusion solution was switched to one containing

MNI-glutamate and the ‘real’ stimulation was performed (supplementary

Figure 3.10).

3.4.2 Single spine LTP has an increased longevity following HSP

We applied an E-LTP stimulus to single visually identified spines in CA1 cells of

the hippocampus, and compared the spine volume of the stimulated spine to the

neighbouring spines. We found that glutamate uncaging induced a structural

potentiation over the two hour time-course in both the control and the TTX

condition (Figure 3.2 a & b, p < 0.001, 2-way rm ANOVA). The maintenance of

LTP after HSP shows that the capacity of the system to encode information

through Hebbian plasticity remains intact. The LTP magnitudes of the two

Page 118: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

100

Figure 3.2 LTP magnitude and longevity following HSP

a) and b) Control and TTX-incubated stimulated and neighbouring spine volumes. Both

conditions show significant structural LTP (p < 0.001) of the stimulated spines compared to

neighbours (2 way repeated measures ANOVA, n = 17 stimulated spines 85 neighbours

(control), 18 stimulated spines, 102 neighbours (TTX). c) The magnitude of LTP is not

different between control and TTX conditions (p > 0.05, 2 way rm ANOVA). d) – f).

Quantifications of the time-courses in parts a) – c). d) Control stimulated spines are

significantly larger than neighbours at timepoint 35-45 minutes (p<0.001) but not at 110-

120 minutes (p>0.05). e) TTX stimulated spines are significantly larger than neighbours at

both 35-45 (p<0.001) and 110-120 minutes (p<0.01). f) No significant differences in LTP

magnitude are seen between stimulated spines in the two conditions. All statistics for parts

d) - e) are t-tests.

Page 119: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

101

conditions were statistically indistinguishable from one another over the whole

time-course (Figure 3.2 c, p > 0.05, 2-way repeated-measures ANOVA). There is

a trend for the TTX magnitude to be greater than the control, but this does not

reach significance. This result differs from the previously reported increase in

LTP magnitude following HSP. A possible explanation for this difference is that

our protocol induces LTP at the level of an individual spine, instead of

simultaneously at a population of synapses as in the previous studies. We can

therefore conclude that the LTP magnitude at single spines is equivalent

independently of HSP expression. We next verified whether the longevity of the

LTP expressed in both cases was the same, by comparing the magnitude of LTP

expressed at 45 minutes (at which growth from E-LTP should be present) and

120 minutes (by which time only spines which have undergone L-LTP will

remain larger) with the respective sizes of the neighbouring spines. As expected

for the E-LTP protocol, we saw that in the control condition there was significant

LTP at 45 minutes, but this had returned to the level of neighbouring spines by

120 minutes (Figure 3.2 d). Surprisingly, LTP in the TTX-stimulated spines was

maintained until the 120 hour timepoint (Figure 3.2 e). This implies that

although the magnitude of LTP is not different between the conditions, the

longevity of the LTP induced is greater after HSP.

3.4.3 Short-term induction of LTP is stronger after HSP

Since HSP is known to cause insertion of AMPA receptors into the spine, the

increased longevity of LTP may be caused by stronger signalling at the time of

stimulation, which therefore induces a mechanistically different and longer

lasting form of LTP. To further investigate this hypothesis, we concentrated on

the dynamics of the plasticity induction. The growth dynamics of stimulated

spines show two non-continuous phases. The first is a very fast, high magnitude

growth (seen by the large peak at timepoint 0 following stimulation, Figure 3.2 a

- c). The second, from the 5 minute timepoint onwards, is a slow relaxation of the

Page 120: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

102

Figure 3.3 Spine growth dynamics during stimulation

a) Spine volume at timepoint 0 shows a trend to be higher in TTX than control spines,

although the effect does not reach significance (p = 0.189, t-test) b) The spine volume at

timepoint 0 is correlated with the final spine volume (i.e. the final magnitude of LTP). TTX

spines showed a stronger correlation (R2 = 0.683) than the control spines (R2 = 0.175) N = 17

control, 18 TTX. c) Representative image of spine growth during the 60 seconds of glutamate

uncaging. d) Individual traces of spine growth during glutamate uncaging stimulation. The

time of individual uncaging pulses are represented in blue at the bottom of the graph. There

was no significant difference between the overall growth curves between control and TTX (p

> 0.05, 2-way rm ANOVA). N = 16 control, 18 TTX. e) Growth between volume as imaged at

the end of stimulation (i.e. at 60 seconds through the stimulation) and volume at the next

imaging point (timepoint 0), approximately 1 minute later. TTX spines show a greater

increase between these two points, meaning these spines continue to grow more after the

end of stimulation (p < 0.05, t-test). N = 16 Control, 18 TTX.

Page 121: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

103

LTP down to a steady state value at approximately 45 minutes. This initial

growth, similar to post-tetanic potentiation (PTP) as is reported for

electrophysiologically-induced LTP (Bao et al., 1997), is a reliable structural

consequence of uncaging which has been reported previously for LTP

stimulations (Matsuzaki et al., 2004). It is likely caused by short-term plasticity

effects, and is gradually lost over the next hour as the plasticity settles to a

steady-state value. We hypothesised that the size of the PTP induced by the

stimulation is an indicator of the success of the uncaging induction; and thus the

amount of growth seen at time-point 0 should be reflective of the final

magnitude of LTP. Indeed, in agreement with the finding that TTX-spines show a

longer-lasting plasticity, the magnitude of the PTP is larger in the TTX spines

than the control, although this result did not reach significance (Figure 3.3 a, p =

0.189).

We then compared the initial spine volume at timepoint 0 to the final volume, to

test our hypothesis that the magnitude of the PTP was predictive of the final LTP

outcome. Indeed, we observed that there was a positive correlation between

these factors for both the TTX-incubated and the control condition (Figure 3.3

b). Interestingly, the correlation was stronger for TTX slices than for control

slices (r2 = 0.687 for TTX, 0.178 for controls).

We also examined the dynamics of the growth throughout the 60 second

stimulation itself, by recording high-speed ROIs (at approximately 20 Hz)

throughout stimulus delivery (Figure 3.3 c). We saw that for spines in both

conditions, growth only started occurring after approximately 15 seconds

(equivalent to 7 or 8 uncaging pulses) and continued throughout the stimulation

period in most but not all of the spines (Figure 3.3 d). There was no significant

difference in the growth curves between the control and TTX spines (Figure 3.3

d, p > 0.05, 2-way repeated measures ANOVA), indicating that HPS expression

has not affected the immediate dynamics of LTP induction. However, the growth

Page 122: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

104

producing the PTP may not end immediately with the stimulation but could

continue up until the next timepoint imaged (approximately 1 minute later),

which is designated timepoint 0. We therefore compared the size at the end of

the 60 seconds of stimulation to the size at timepoint 0 to see whether indeed

growth continued. In the control case, spines do not continue to grow after the

end of stimulation, but the TTX spines do indeed show growth between these

two timepoints (Figure 3.3 e, p < 0.05, t-test). This suggests that that the

stimulus is having a longer lasting effect in the TTX case than in the control,

which could be the result of changes in receptor composition or short-term

plasticity mechanisms, such as increased calcium influx, which prolong the signal

from the glutamate and lead to enhanced signalling.

3.4.4 The proportion of spines that show LTP is increased after HSP

After determining that LTP induction appears to be stronger in the HSP case, we

next looked at how successful the induction of LTP was. Our stimulus is not

100% efficacious but instead generates a certain amount of failures which do not

result in LTP. In line with current theories of LTP longevity we chose to use the

period between 35 - 45 minutes as the point where, if induced, LTP should be

apparent (regardless of whether it would prove to be E-LTP or L-LTP at later

timepoints). We classified a successful LTP induction as one where the spine size

was significantly different to the baseline values in the timepoints between 30 -

45 minutes, using a standard t-test (Figure 3.4 a & b). We discovered that in the

control condition we induced LTP in 59% of spines, which was increased to 78%

in the TTX condition (Figure 3.4 c). This suggests that, as we had hypothesised

from the data in Figure 3.3, the induction of LTP is more efficacious after HSP.

Page 123: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

105

Figure 3.4 LTP induction had increased efficacy following HSP

a) All control experiments and b) All TTX experiments for LTP induction, showing stimulated

spine (dark grey and red respectively) and neighbours (light grey and pink respectively).

Neighbours are plotted as mean + SEM. The timepoint used to determine successful LTP (30-

45 minutes) is marked with a line. Successful induction of LTP is marked by a dashed grey

line; unsuccessful induction is marked by a solid maroon line. 10 out of 17 (59%) of control

spines show LTP, compared to 14 out of 18 (78% of TTX spines). c) Percentage LTP induction

successes and failures for the 2 conditions.

Page 124: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

106

Figure 3.5 Small spines show greater LTP after HSP

a) Linear regression of spine sizes vs. the magnitude of growth at 45 minutes. The TTX

condition shows a significant negative correlation (r2 = 0.27, p = 0.027), whilst the control

shows a non-significant negative trend (r2 = 0.13, p = 0.158). b) Spine sizes vs. magnitude of

LTP at 120 minutes. The TTX case shows an increased negative correlation (r2 = 0.48, p =

0.002), whilst in the control case the negative trend has all but disappeared (r2 = 0.06, p =

Page 125: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

107

3.4.5 Small spines show a greater LTP than large spines, which is

enhanced by HSP

Knowing that there was a difference in the proportions of spines that potentiate

between TTX and control conditions, we sought to understand what parameters

determine whether or not a spine can express plasticity. One factor that has been

shown to influence the capacity for structural plasticity is the initial spine

volume. Small spines have a greater tendency to potentiate and grow, whereas

large spines express less structural LTP during weak forms of plasticity

(Matsuzaki et al., 2004; Paulin et al., 2016). This result is unsurprising from the

point of view of resource availability, since it would require many more absolute

cellular resources for a large spine to double its volume than for a small one. It

also raises the intriguing possibility that there is a ceiling for potentiation, above

which a spine can’t grow. This maximum set-point could be a biological

imperative. Alternatively, it could be a consequence of the mechanisms required

for plasticity, because when the volume is very large, it may be impossible for

plasticity-inducing signals such as calcium to reach the requisite concentration to

induce further LTP (Sabatini et al., 2001). We looked for a connection between

the initial size of the spine and the plasticity it expressed, at 45 minutes (as a

correlate of E-LTP) and 120 minutes (as a correlate of L-LTP), in our two

conditions. At 45 minutes, as predicted from previous studies, there was a

negative correlation between the initial spine volume and the expressed

plasticity in both conditions (Figure 3.5 a). Unexpectedly there was a stronger

0.346). c) Distribution of all initial volumes of stimulated spines, with example spine images.

The cut-off for defining ‘large’ spines was the median volume off all spines multiplied by 1.5.

Scale bar = 1 µm. d) Time-course of the experiment plotting only the small spines. TTX-

incubated spines show a significantly increased magnitude of LTP (p < 0.05, 2-way repeated

measures ANOVA) than the control. N = 13 control, 11 TTX. e) Time-course of LTP plotting

only large spines. There is no difference between the volumes of TTX and control spines. N = 4

control, 7 TTX.

Page 126: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

108

correlation for the TTX-incubated condition than for the control condition (r2 =

0.27 for TTX, p < 0.05, r2 = 0.13 for control, p > 0.05). This trend was amplified by

the 120 hour timepoint (Figure 3.5 b), where the correlation had grown even

tighter for the TTX case (r2 = 0.48, p < 0.01), but had disappeared for the control

(r2 = 0.06, p > 0.3). Since the size dependence of LTP was clearly modulated by

the expression of HSP, we divided the stimulated spines into either ‘small’ or

‘large’ spines. Spines were classified as ‘large’ if their initial volume was more

than 150% of the median initial volume of all the spines (Figure 3.5 c). We then

plotted the whole time-course of LTP separately for the small and large spines.

When only the small spines were considered, there was a significant increase in

the magnitude of LTP in the TTX condition as compared to the controls (Figure

3.5 d) (p < 0.05, 2-way rm ANOVA). Large spines showed a negligible amount of

LTP with no difference between the two conditions (Figure 3.5 e). Thus the

capacity for specifically small spines to express LTP after undergoing HSP is

increased.

Although small spines undoubtedly show more plasticity after HSP, we also

know from the data presented in Chapter 2 of this thesis that spines on average

grow after 48 hours of TTX application, so small spines will be rarer in this

condition than in the control. The spines selected for uncaging were not picked

at random, but were in fact chosen to span a wide range of sizes, to allow us to

understand how size affects plasticity. Could it be that in the typical distribution

of spines after TTX application, those we have designated ‘small’ are in fact

extremely rare, and so their enhanced plasticity will be near-to-irrelevant for the

functioning of the circuit under normal circumstances? To address this question,

we used the population data of all spines sizes presented in Figure 2.4 from

Chapter 2. From these measurements we calculated the 25th, 50t and 75th

percentiles for spine sizes in each of the two conditions (Figure 3.6 a). We

observed that none of the stimulated spines in either condition fell below

the25% percentile, so even the smallest of our stimulated spines are not

Page 127: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

109

Figure 3.6 Behaviour of different sized spines during LTP

a) Population data from Figure 2.4, which was used to define percentiles of population

volumes. Beneath the population data is plotted the initial sizes of the spines used for

uncaging experiments. b) – d) Time-courses of LTP experiments for stimulated spines

separated by initial volumes. b) Stimulated spines in the 25th – 50th percentile. N = 2 control,

6 TTX. c) Stimulated spines in the 50th – 75th percentile. N = 7 control, 4 TTX. d) Stimulated

spines in the 75th – 100th percentile. N = 8 control, 8 TTX.

Page 128: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

110

vanishingly rare in the whole population. We then plotted the LTP time-course

for the percentiles separately (Figure 3.6 b – e). We saw that, as suggested by

the data in Figure 3.5 d, there is a clear difference between the magnitude of

LTP for spines between the 25th and 50th percentiles (Figure 3.6 b). This trend

did not reach significance, likely because the N was very low (2 for the control, 6

for the TTX). For all of the other percentile graphs, there was no difference

between the control and TTX spines (Figure 3.6 c – e). We can therefore

conclude that up to half of the spines in the population, those below the 50th

percentile, show an enhanced LTP after HSP, and so this effect is not so rare as to

be irrelevant. When the different percentiles are plotted together, there is a clear

relationship between the spine volume and the eventual outcome of plasticity for

the TTX spines (supplementary Figure 3.11 b), but no obvious relationship in

the control case (supplementary Figure 3.11 a). This result reinforces the

conclusion from Figure 3.5 a & b, i.e. that spine size is extremely predictive of

the efficacy of LTP after HSP, but not in the control condition.

3.4.6 Proximal neighbouring spines show short-term structural

potentiation after HSP

As described earlier, the stimulated spines in both experimental conditions show

significant structural LTP in comparison to neighbouring non-stimulated spines.

The majority of studies involving single spine plasticity report that there is no

effect of glutamate uncaging at single spines on nearby neighbours (Matsuzaki et

al., 2004; Harvey and Svoboda, 2007; Govindarajan et al., 2011). However, a

recent study reported that LTP at multiple spines simultaneously caused

shrinkage of nearby non-stimulated spines, hinting that complex dynamics may

be at play with regards to resource use and plasticity expression at neighbouring

spines (Oh et al., 2015). We decided to investigate whether the HSP alters

neighbouring spine dynamics after the induction of single-spine LTP.

Page 129: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

111

The first indication that there might be an effect on neighbouring spines came

from ranking all of the spines by their distance from the stimulated spine (with 1

being the closest to the stimulated spine) and plotting their growth dynamics

over the 2 hour experiment (Figure 3.7 a). There was a small but noticeable

increase in the volumes of neighbouring spines in close proximity to the

stimulated spine in the TTX case, which is not observed in the control (compare

the first 20 spines in each case, from 0 minutes to 60 minutes after stimulation).

To examine this effect further, we divided neighbours into those closer than 5

µm from the stimulated spine (proximal), and those further away (distal). When

we plotted the time-course over the whole experiments in these two conditions,

we observed that whilst the control neighbours were indistinguishable in the

two conditions (Figure 3.7 b), the TTX proximal neighbours were significantly

different from the distal neighbours over the whole time-course (Figure 3.7 c),

(p < 0.05, 2-way rm ANOVA). This difference was especially prominent

immediately after stimulus delivery and for the succeeding 20-30 minutes. We

conclude that after HSP the neighbours are being affected by the glutamate

uncaging stimulus, even though they are not the spines being directly targeted.

Close examination of the neighbouring dynamics revealed that proximal TTX

neighbours showed a structural post-tetanic potentiation (PTP) at timepoint 0 in

a qualitatively similar manner to that which was observed in stimulated spines,

although with a smaller magnitude. Intriguingly, distal TTX neighbours showed a

slight depression at this timepoint and for the subsequent 5-10 minutes (Figure

3.7 d). This led us to propose that proximal spines may be positively correlated

to the growth dynamics of the stimulated spine; whereas distal spines may be

negatively correlated. To test this we calculated the correlation coefficient

between the dynamics of the stimulated spine and of the neighbouring spines. If

the spine dynamics were highly positively correlated the correlation coefficient

would be close to 1, whereas negative correlations would be close to -1, and

completely uncorrelated dynamics would yield 0. We indeed found that, whilst

Page 130: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

112

Figure 3.7 Neighbouring spines respond to stimulation after HSP

a) Heat maps of growth dynamics of neighbouring spines over time. Neighbours from all

experiments were pooled and ranked in order of how far they were from the stimulated

Page 131: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

113

there was no relationship between distance and correlation coefficient in the

control condition (Figure 3.7 d), the correlation coefficient for TTX neighbours

was highly dependent on the distance from it to the stimulated spine (Figure 3.7

f, p < 0.001). Neighbours proximal to the stimulated spine had a high correlation,

which then fell with increasing distance to reach negative values at > 10 µm from

the spine. This intriguing result implies there are some competitive mechanisms

at play which mean that growth (and therefore resource use) of the proximal

spines causes depletion of the resources for distal spines, causing them to shrink.

Other spine parameters were uncorrelated to distance, such as initial spine

volume (supplementary Figure 3.12 a & b) or final spine volume

(supplementary Figure 3.12 c). This shows that there was no bias to the spines

picked to be analysed which could explain the structural effects we see on

proximal neighbours.

spine. These were then plotted in order, with index 1 being the spine closest to its stimulated

spine, and the final index being the spine furthest away. Colours represent the normalised

volume of the spine, from 0 to 5. N = 85 spines, 17 dendrites (Control); 102 spines, 18

dendrites (TTX). b) & c) Spines were divided into proximal (< 5 µm from the stimulated

spine) and distal (> 5 µm). b) Control neighbouring spines show no difference between

proximal and distal spines. n = 22 proximal, 63 distal. c) TTX neighbouring spines are

significantly different between proximal and distal (p < 0.05, 2-way rm ANOVA. n = 32

proximal, 70 distal). d) The average volume over the first 10 minutes after stimulation for

the different neighbouring conditions. Control neighbours show no growth and no difference

to one another (p > 0.05), whilst TTX proximal neighbours are significantly larger than distal

(p < 0.001, Kruskal-Wallis post-hoc Dunn. n = 22 control proximal, 63 control distal, 32 TTX

proximal, 70 TTX distal). e) & f) Correlation coefficients were calculated between the

stimulated spine and the neighbouring spines. This was regressed against the distance from

the stimulated spine. e) There is no relationship between distance and correlation

coefficient for the control neighbours (p > 0.05). f) TTX neighbours show a strong

relationship between distance and correlation coefficient (p < 0.001).

Page 132: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

114

3.4.7 The threshold for plasticity induction is reduced following

HSP

To test the hypothesis that HSP sensitises spines to allow them to respond to

lower glutamate levels, we devised a new stimulus protocol with a reduced

strength. Based on reported protocols in the literature, this employs a shortened

laser pulse width of 1 ms (compared to 4 ms for the previous protocol), so

lowering the amount of glutamate released during the uncaging stimulus

(Harvey and Svoboda, 2007; Govindarajan et al., 2011). All other parameters of

the stimulus, i.e. stimulus power, frequency and duration, remained the same.

This ‘sub-threshold’ protocol has been described as having no long-lasting

structural effects on spines when applied to one spine. However, when applied in

conjunction with other strong LTP stimuli, the spine can respond through either

a process of cross-talk or synaptic cooperation for proteins. We applied the 1 ms

protocol to individual spines in both control and TTX conditions, and followed

the resulting spine volume for 1 hour post stimulus. We observed that in the

control condition, there is a short-term PTP following the stimulus but no long

lasting plasticity. The growth dynamics are not significantly different from the

neighbouring spines over the entire time-course (Figure 3.8 a, p > 0.05, 2-way

repeated measures ANOVA, post hoc Bonferroni). The initial PTP, which is not

reported in previous data using this protocol, may be attributable to a variation

in our protocol by which we ensure that Mg2+ levels in the uncaging aCSF are

absolute zero, to allow for maximal stimulation of NMDA receptors. The

previously reported protocols, whilst having no added Mg2+ in the uncaging

aCSF, likely had low levels of residual Mg2+ from the preceding perfusion of the

normal aCSF (personal communication). This will have reduced the likelihood of

NMDA channel opening and so decreased the stimulus strength. In the TTX

condition, in contrast to the control, the stimulated spine grows significantly

when compared to neighbouring spines over the 1 hour observation period

(Figure 3.8 b, p < 0.01, 2-way rm ANOVA, post hoc Bonferroni). Thus as we

Page 133: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

115

hypothesised the threshold for the glutamate level needed to induce LTP was

lowered by the previous expression of HSP. We then compared the magnitudes

of growth after the 1 ms protocol compared to our previous data from the 4 ms

protocol. In the control condition as expected there was a significant difference

between the 1 ms and 4 ms conditions (Figure 3.8 c, p > 0.05). In the TTX case

Figure 3.8 Reduced threshold for LTP after HSP

a) A sub-threshold stimulus with 1 ms pulse width produces a PTP but no long lasting

potentiation in the control condition (p > 0.05, 2-way repeated measures ANOVA post-hoc

Bonferroni) N = 11 stimulated spines, 52 neighbouring spines. b) A 1 ms stimulus does

produce LTP after TTX incubation (p < 0.01). N = 11 stimulated spines, 52 neighbouring

spines. c) The structural changes are significantly reduced after the 1 ms stimulus as

compared to the usual 4 ms LTP stimulus in the control condition (4 ms data is the same as is

shown in Figure 3.2). (p < 0.05 2-way rm ANOVA). N = 11 for 1 ms, 17 for 4 ms. d) There is

no significant difference between the 4 ms and 1 ms traces after TTX incubation. (p > 0.05m

2-way rm ANOVA, although they are different at the 0 minutes timepoint, p < 0.001, post-hoc

Bonferroni). N = 11 for 1 ms, 18 for 4 ms.

Page 134: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

116

however the 4 ms and the 1 ms were not significantly different, showing that this

nominally sub-threshold stimulus is in fact supra-threshold after HSP (Figure

3.8 d, p > 0.05). The two traces did however differ at the 0 minute timepoint (p <

0.001), implying that the induction of plasticity was stronger after the 4 ms

stimulus. Since we previously described in Figure 3.3 that the induction size was

predictive of the final outcome of plasticity, it would be interesting to repeat the

1 ms experiments but follow the spines for 2 hours instead of 1 hour as we

present here. We would predict that lower magnitude of induction than in the 4

ms case would be translated into shorter-lasting plasticity, probably being lost

before the 2 hour timepoint.

3.4.8 LTP capacity is maintained after 72 hours of activity blockade

Although we chose to conduct the majority of our LTP experiments after 48

hours of activity blockade with TTX, we know from the electrophysiology data

presented in Chapter 2 that, even after 72 hours incubation with TTX, neurons

are still healthy and showing normal electrophysiological responses. We

therefore decided to conduct a set of LTP experiments at 72 hours, to verify

whether the capacity for LTP was maintained even after so long without activity.

Importantly, the experiments previously described which showed an increased

magnitude of functional LTP after HSP were performed after 60 hours of activity

blockade (Arendt et al., 2013). Conducting experiments at 72 hours would allow

us to span the timeframe of these previous experiments and verify that any

differences we observed between ours and their results were not due to a change

in the system responses occurring between 48 hours and 60 hours of activity

blockade. When we performed the uncaging experiments after 72 hours of

activity blockade, we saw that LTP was indeed induced at this timepoint, and

that the magnitude was indistinguishable from that of the control slices (Figure

3.9). We therefore concluded that we saw no evidence for increased magnitude

Page 135: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

117

of LTP at 72 hours, so the difference between our data and that seen in Arendt et

al is not due to the timepoint, but is instead more likely to be attributable to the

differences between field stimulation and single spine stimulation.

Figure 3.9 Capacity for LTP is maintained after 72 hours of activity block

The time-courses for the progression of LTP after 72 hours of activity blockade are

indistinguishable for the control and the TTX conditions. N = 6 control, 3 TTX

Page 136: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

118

3.5 Discussion

In this chapter, we have shown that single spine LTP is intact and has an

increased longevity following 48 hours of activity blockade (Figure 3.2). These

data demonstrate that the information-coding capacity of the circuit is not

compromised by the expression of HSP. Since many of the output mechanisms

are the same between HSP and LTP, we may have expected the opposite – that

the expression of HSP to occludes LTP. Instead, the cells and spines retain their

ability to further undergo plasticity. Indeed, not only is LTP still possible but it is

even enhanced, as seen by an increased efficacy of induction (Figure 3.3 &

Figure 3.4). Small spines in particular show a higher LTP magnitude after

activity blockade (Figure 3.5). An explanation for this may be that HSP lowers

the threshold for potentiation, shown by metaplasticity at neighbouring spines

after HSP (Figure 3.7) and an increased response to a stimulus with lower

glutamate levels (Figure 3.8).

3.5.1 Correlation between spine size and LTP after HSP

An interesting aspect of our results was that spine size is extremely predictive of

the eventual outcome of LTP after TTX-mediated activity blockade, but not in the

control condition. Although it is not possible to know from our data what causes

this, we can speculate on a possible explanation, which is that magnitude of LTP

is determined not just by the uncaging stimulation but also by the previous

history of the spine. Recent plasticity may not affect the immediate growth

during the stimulation, which is dependent on contemporaneous calcium

signalling caused by activation of receptors, but it may modulate the long-term

outcome of plasticity due to residual plasticity-related products. Indeed not only

plasticity events at the spine itself but those occurring at a different spine in the

same dendritic domain can also affect the long-term plasticity of the spine

through processes of cross-talk mediated by diffusion of the signalling molecule

Ras (Harvey and Svoboda, 2007; Harvey et al., 2008) or sharing of proteins

Page 137: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

119

through synaptic tagging and co-operation (Frey and Morris, 1997; Govindarajan

et al., 2011). In the control case for our experiments, the spines have been

undergoing activity (and so by extension, activity-dependent plasticity)

throughout their development, so the uncaging stimulus and the growth caused

by it are only partially instructive of the final magnitude of plasticity. In the TTX

case however activity has been blocked for the preceding 48 hours, meaning that

there are no residual effects from other plasticity events and the uncaging

stimulus is very predictive of the final plasticity expressed.

3.5.2 Volume dependence of LTP expression

Enhanced LTP in small spines after HSP is a novel and interesting result for the

field of homeostatic synaptic plasticity. It suggests that although synaptic

strengths scale up proportionally after HSP, the potential for plasticity may be

selectively enhanced at small spines as compared to large ones. A clue to why

this might happen could lie in the data presented in Chapter 2. There we saw that

at the 48 hour timepoint, spines did not scale linearly but instead there was an

over-representation of large spines (Figure 2.4). This suggests that large spines

undergo homeostatic potentiation before small spines, and thus increase supra-

linearly. By 72 hours however, the distribution has re-linearised, which could be

achieved by the small spines having ‘caught up’ and themselves undergone

potentiation. The timepoint we studied here, 48 hours after activity block, may

therefore be one in which small spines are especially primed to structurally

potentiate. Homeostatic mechanisms have already occurred to strengthen the

synapses (as shown by the linear functional scaling seen in Figure 2.3), but the

structure has not yet stabilised to match. The result is a ‘gain modulation’ of

Hebbian plasticity – small spines are in a state of readiness which allows an

easier induction and increased longevity of LTP when they receive the

appropriate stimulus. This could act to make the circuit more sensitive to any

inputs it does receive. Large spines will already have a high conductance so their

Page 138: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

120

potentiation will not greatly change the circuit. Small spines however will

initially contribute little to the activity of the post-synaptic cell, so increasing

their capacity to potentiate can allow a neuron to respond to any activity more

easily. This possibility coheres well with the hypothesis put forward in Chapter

2, whereby activity blockade equalises network firing rates, disrupting the

normal skewed firing distribution. In an extreme case of activity perturbation

such as total activity blockade, the normal circuit architecture is clearly

insufficient to provide the necessary input for the network. It therefore makes

sense to override the previously determined circuit level firing, to attempt to find

a new network shape which can maintain the correct levels of firing. The same

argument can be put forth for the synapses on a single cell. Whilst synaptic

scaling will preserve weights and thus the learned shape of the network, in an

extreme case that architecture is clearly unhelpful, since the spines that

previously transferred a lot of network activity are now failing to do so. In such

cases, a cell needs to acquire alternate sources of upstream activity to maintain

the correct levels. This activity may come from sources which previously it did

not have strong links to – i.e. the small synapses. By priming the small synapses

to potentiate for lower levels of activity and to a higher magnitude, the cell

increases its chances of re-entering a functional network and restoring its

optimal activity levels.

3.5.3 Mechanisms for enhanced LTP after HSP

Previous studies of LTP following HSP have provided possible mechanistic

explanations for the results we observe in this chapter. A likely candidate for

mediating the changes in plasticity capacity is the NMDA receptor, which is

highly involved in plasticity due to its functioning as a coincidence detector and

its calcium permeability. The affect of activity changes on the expression of

NMDA channels in not unanimously agreed upon, with some reports of no

change in NMDA expression following HSP (Lissin et al., 1998) and others

Page 139: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

121

showing increased NMDA expression (Watt et al., 2000). Despite this, there is a

weight of evidence to suggest that even though the number of NMDARs may not

change, their distribution and subunit composition is altered by activity blockade

(Lee et al., 2010; Arendt et al., 2013). Blockade of single synapses using a genetic

tool to prevent pre-synaptic vesicle fusion caused an increase in NMDA receptors

containing the GluR2B (as opposed to the GluR2A) subunit (Lee et al., 2010). This

is a particularly relevant change because the GluR2B subunit has an increased

calcium permeability, as well as having an increased affinity with other signalling

components important for structural plasticity such as CaMKII (Barria and

Malinow, 2005; Paoletti et al., 2013). An increased calcium influx and prolonged

signalling dynamics could be responsible for the data presented in Figure 3.3,

which showed that for the same amount of glutamate stimulation, the TTX-

incubated spines grew for longer periods of time (even after the stimulation had

finished) and reached higher levels than the control spines. It would be

interesting in the future to use a calcium sensor such as GCaMP6 to observe the

calcium dynamics during and immediately following the stimulation, to test the

hypothesis that calcium signalling is prolonged after HSP leading to a longer

lasting plasticity. This subunit change may also make the synapse responsive to

lower glutamate levels, since less NMDAR activation is required for the same

amount of calcium signalling, which could explain the data from Figure 3.7 and

Figure 3.8.

A related recent finding is the reported increase in silent (i.e. AMPAR-lacking)

synapses after HSP induction (Arendt et al., 2013). Since these synapses are

newly formed they are likely to be small, and so could plausibly be the ‘small’

synapses we observe showing enhanced LTP in our TTX-incubated slices

(although it’s worth noting that in control conditions there is no correlation

between occurrence of silent synapses and spine age (Zito et al., 2009)). It is

currently unknown how a silent synapse will respond to single-synapse LTP. In

conditions of physiological extra-cellular Mg2+, the activation of NMDA receptors

Page 140: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

122

requires post-synaptic depolarisation to relieve the Mg2+ block, so it is unlikely

that glutamate stimulation of a silent synapse alone could cause plasticity. In our

experimental system where an extracellular concentration of 0 mM Mg2+ releases

the Mg2+ block, NMDA receptors will be activated by single synapse stimulation,

and thus a silent synapse could still undergo structural and functional LTP. Using

glutamate uncaging it is possible to stimulate multiple spines pseudo-

synchronously to cause the intra-cellular depolarisation necessary for Mg2+ block

release whilst in the presence of physiological concentrations of Mg2+ (Losonczy

and Magee, 2006; Losonczy et al., 2008; Govindarajan et al., 2011). It would be

very interesting to repeat these experiments in this chapter in physiological

concentrations of Mg2+ but stimulating multiple synapses, to see whether the

same results are obtained for enhanced small spine growth. To directly test the

capacity for plasticity of silent synapses, one could use a voltage-dependent dye

to fill the neuron. Imaging a synapse while performing glutamate uncaging at it

would reveal whether the stimulation produced a depolarisation. If no

depolarisation could be observed, the synapse would be lacking AMPAR and

would therefore be silent. The plasticity-inducing experiments detailed in this

chapter could then be carried out as usual.

3.5.4 Heterosynaptic plasticity after HSP

We observed that spine close the stimulated spine exhibited a transient

structural LTP in the TTX condition, but not the control. There are two

mechanistic possibilities for why proximal TTX spines are affected by the

uncaging stimulus, whilst distal ones are not. The first is that glutamate is

spilling over from the stimulation site and reaching neighbouring spines. In this

case, HSP must have sensitised the spines to respond to extremely low levels of

glutamate, since we do not see any effect in the control condition. This is unlikely

given what is known about the extremely small excitation volume of glutamate

uncaging (Pettit et al., 1997; Smith et al., 2003). The second more likely

Page 141: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

123

possibility is that plasticity-induced signalling within the dendrite is spreading

from the stimulated spine to the regions of neighbouring spines and causing

structural plasticity without the neighbouring synapse itself receiving glutamate

stimulation. It is known that plasticity-related signalling molecules such as Ras

can indeed spread to nearby dendritic regions and lower the threshold for

plasticity, a process known as cross-talk (Harvey and Svoboda, 2007; Harvey et

al., 2008). With our current data it is very difficult to distinguish whether either

(or both) of these two possibilities is occurring, since any nearby spines will be

close both in dendritic distance and in space for glutamate diffusion. An

empirical way to distinguish between the two would be to stimulate a spine

which had visible neighbours very close by but on a separate branch, so the

physical distance for glutamate to diffuse is small but the dendritic distance is

very large. If glutamate spill-over was the reason for the increased neighbouring

growth, we would expect these spines to show plasticity. If a signal in the

dendrite causes the threshold lowering, these spines should not be affected. It

will be extremely interesting to know if one or both of these mechanisms are

playing a role in the effect on neighbours we have observed. I will discuss the

implications of this type of heterosynaptic plasticity in greater detail in Chapter

4, the general discussion.

3.5.5 Conclusion

It is clear that much more work remains to be done to fully understand how

Hebbian plasticity and HSP interact. We here have uncovered a feature of HSP

which confers an increased sensitivity to LTP on the neural circuit, therefore

allowing it to respond more strongly to any inputs it receives, which opens the

door to deeper investigation of the molecular and structural mechanisms which

allow this to occur.

Page 142: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

124

3.6 Supplementary figures

Figure 3.10 Post-stimulation growth for real stimulations and sham stimulations

Sham stimulations were delivered to all neurons at the beginning of the experiment. Only if

no growth was seen due to the sham stimulus was the experiment continued to deliver the

real stimulation. Spine volumes after the real stimulations are significantly higher than for

either the neighbouring case, or the sham stimulation (p < 0.001, ANOVA post-hoc

Bonferroni).

Figure 3.11 LTP magnitude grouped by spine volume

a) Control stimulated spines divided into percentiles of spine volume. There is no clear

relationship between the volume of the spine and the eventual magnitude of LTP which it

expresses. b) TTX stimulated spines. After activity blockade, the spine volume is very

predictive of the magnitude of the LTP throughout the whole experimental time-course.

Page 143: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

125

Figure 3.12 Neighbouring spine parameters

a) There was no relationship between the distance from the stimulated spine and the initial

spine volume, showing that there was no bias in the spines selected for analysis which could

account for the neighbouring dynamics seen in Figure 3.7. TTX neighbours are on average

slightly larger than control neighbours, as would be expected from the data presented in

Chapter 2 Figure 2.4. b) Linear regressions of the initial spine volume with the normalised

final spine volume. Although there is a slight negative trend, there was no significant

correlation for either the control or TTX condition (p > 0.05). c) There is no relationship

between the distance from the stimulated spine and the final spine volume for either

condition (p < 0.05), showing that any structural changes which occur due to the stimulus are

short-lasting.

Page 144: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

126

3.7 Bibliography

Arendt KL, Sarti F, Chen L (2013) Chronic inactivation of a neural circuit enhances LTP by inducing silent synapse formation. J Neurosci 33:2087–2096.

Argunsah AO, Erdil E, Ghani M., Yagci AM, Ramiro-Cortes Y, Hobbiss AF, Kanik S., Cetin M, Israely I, Unay D (2016) SpineS: A Tool for Automatic Tracking of Dendritic Spines. “In prep.”

Bao J-XX, Kandel ER, Hawkins RD (1997) Involvement of Pre- and Postsynaptic Mechanisms in Posttetanic Potentiation at Aplysia Synapses. Science (80- ) 275:969–973.

Barria A, Malinow R (2005) NMDA receptor subunit composition controls synaptic plasticity by regulating binding to CaMKII. Neuron 48:289–301.

Busetto G, Higley MJ, Sabatini BL (2008) Developmental presence and disappearance of postsynaptically silent synapses on dendritic spines of rat layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons. J Physiol 586:1519–1527.

Denk W, Strickler JH, Webb WW (1990) Two-photon laser scanning fluorescence microscopy. Science 248:73–76.

Félix-Oliveira A, Dias RB, Colino-Oliveira M, Rombo DM, Sebastião AM (2014) Homeostatic plasticity induced by brief activity deprivation enhances long-term potentiation in the mature rat hippocampus. J Neurophysiol 112:3012–3022.

Fonseca R, Nägerl UV, Morris RGM, Bonhoeffer T (2004) Competing for memory: hippocampal LTP under regimes of reduced protein synthesis. Neuron 44:1011–1020.

Frey U, Morris R (1997) Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature.

Govindarajan A, Israely I, Huang S-Y, Tonegawa S (2011) The dendritic branch is the preferred integrative unit for protein synthesis-dependent LTP. Neuron 69:132–146.

Harvey CD, Svoboda K (2007) Locally dynamic synaptic learning rules in pyramidal neuron dendrites. Nature 450:1195–1200.

Harvey CD, Yasuda R, Zhong H, Svoboda K (2008) The spread of Ras activity triggered by activation of a single dendritic spine. Science 321:136–140.

Ibata K, Sun Q, Turrigiano GG (2008) Rapid Synaptic Scaling Induced by Changes

Page 145: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

127

in Postsynaptic Firing. Neuron 57:819–826.

Lee M-C, Yasuda R, Ehlers MD (2010) Metaplasticity at Single Glutamatergic Synapses. Neuron 66:859–870.

Lissin D V, Gomperts SN, Carroll RC, Christine CW, Kalman D, Kitamura M, Hardy S, Nicoll RA, Malenka RC, von Zastrow M (1998) Activity differentially regulates the surface expression of synaptic AMPA and NMDA glutamate receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:7097–7102.

Losonczy A, Magee JC (2006) Integrative properties of radial oblique dendrites in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Neuron 50:291–307.

Losonczy A, Makara JK, Magee JC (2008) Compartmentalized dendritic plasticity and input feature storage in neurons. Nature 452:436–441.

Matsuzaki M, Honkura N, Ellis-Davies GCR, Kasai H (2004) Structural basis of long-trm potentiation in single dendritic spines. Nature 429:761–766.

Oh WC, Parajuli LK, Zito K (2015) Heterosynaptic Structural Plasticity on Local Dendritic Segments of Hippocampal CA1 Neurons. Cell Rep 10:162–169.

Palma-Cerda F, Auger C, Crawford DJ, Hodgson ACC, Reynolds SJ, Cowell JK, Swift KAD, Cais O, Vyklicky L, Corrie JET, Ogden D (2012) New caged neurotransmitter analogs selective for glutamate receptor sub-types based on methoxynitroindoline and nitrophenylethoxycarbonyl caging groups. Neuropharmacology 63:624–634.

Paoletti P, Bellone C, Zhou Q (2013) NMDA receptor subunit diversity: impact on receptor properties, synaptic plasticity and disease. Nat Rev Neurosci 14:383–400.

Paulin JJW, Haslehurst P, Fellows AD, Liu W, Jackson JD, Joel Z, Cummings DM, Edwards FA (2016) Large and Small Dendritic Spines Serve Different Interacting Functions in Hippocampal Synaptic Plasticity and Homeostasis. Neural Plast 2016.

Pawlak V, Schupp BJ, Single FN, Seeburg PH, Köhr G (2005) Impaired synaptic scaling in mouse hippocampal neurones expressing NMDA receptors with reduced calcium permeability. J Physiol 562:771–783.

Pettit DL, Wang SSH, Gee KR, Augustine GJ (1997) Chemical two-photon uncaging: A novel approach to mapping glutamate receptors. Neuron 19:465–471.

Sabatini BL, Maravall M, Svoboda K (2001) Ca2+ signaling in dendritic spines.

Page 146: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

128

Curr Opin Neurobiol 11:349–356.

Smith MA, Ellis-Davies GCR, Magee JC (2003) Mechanism of the distance-dependent scaling of Schaffer collateral synapses in rat CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Physiol 548:245–258.

Thiagarajan TC, Lindskog M, Tsien RW (2005) Adaptation to synaptic inactivity in hippocampal neurons. Neuron 47:725–737.

Watt AJ, van Rossum MC, MacLeod KM, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG (2000) Activity coregulates quantal AMPA and NMDA currents at neocortical synapses. Neuron 26:659–670.

Zito K, Scheuss V, Knott G, Hill T, Svoboda K (2009) Rapid functional maturation of nascent dendritic spines. Neuron 61:247–258.

Page 147: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

129

h

Chapter 4

4 General Discussion

General discussion:

Extending HSP from

synapses to network-level

functioning

Page 148: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

130

4.1 Abstract

The need for stability is a universal one in biological systems. In this thesis we

have shown that neurons can co-opt structural mechanisms to prolong and

enhance efforts to normalise their activity. In this discussion we explore how the

structural changes, which seem to be decoupled from functional ones, will affect

the circuit functioning. In particular, we focus on how the selective sensitisation

of small spines will alter the arrangement of the network, and how this might be

reinforced by known plasticity interactions such as synaptic tagging and capture.

We also discuss how the heterosynaptic plasticity observed in neighbouring

spines might contribute to network level changes. Lastly, we discuss how HSP

might play a role in neurodevelopmental disorders, and its connection to sleep

mechanisms as proposed in the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis (SHY).

Page 149: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

131

Before exploring the themes of this thesis, a note about phraseology. When

writing this discussion, the intuitive method to write about the tendency for a

system – be it a cell, synapse or circuit – to stay within optimal bounds is as

‘wanting’ to return. This anthropomorphism is clearly inaccurate, seeing as it

attributes a sense of agency to an individual component of a circuit. Despite that

it is perhaps not misleading, in that it allows us a shorthand way to refer to the

driving force behind the circuit dynamics; even if the biological reasons for that

driving force are not yet entirely known. Thus, a certain number of the terms

‘wanting, ‘preferring’ or such similar phrases may have crept in. I request that

the reader treat these with a sense of leniency in the literal sense, and instead

ask that attention to be concentrated on the idea behind them.

4.2 Overview of work in this thesis

In this thesis, we describe the structural changes which accompany the induction

and expression of homeostatic synaptic plasticity (HSP). We induce HSP by

incubating organotypic slices for 0 – 72 hours in the Na+ channel blocker TTX,

and use electrophysiological methods and imaging to assess the functional and

structural changes which occur as a result of the activity blockade. In common

with previous observations for physiology, we discovered that synapses scaled

upwards after 48 – 72 hours activity blockade. With regards to the structure, and

contrary to predictions, we did not discover a linear scaling relationship after

homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Instead, some spines showed a higher

propensity to co-opt homeostatic mechanisms than others, leading to a

preponderance of large spines after 48 hours of activity blockade. However,

when stimulated individually using glutamate uncaging, these large spines

showed little capacity to further express LTP (as indeed was the case for the

control condition). The small spines, which had not been scaled up by a large

amount, were however highly sensitive to LTP, and grew more than those in the

Page 150: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

132

control condition. Neighbouring spines at nearby dendritic locations to the

stimulated spine also showed a short-term structural plasticity after HSP, but not

in the control condition. This led us to propose a ‘sensitisation’ mechanism,

whereby small spines have an increased capacity to express LTP following HSP.

4.3 Beyond synaptic scaling – silent synapses and network

function

The process of sensitisation at select synapses will benefit a neural system

undergoing HSP by making it able to respond to new sources of activity. In vivo

strong homeostatic mechanisms, such as induced in the experiments for this

thesis, will come into play when a system loses a large amount of input, for

instance after sensory deprivation or after the loss of a limb. The main sources of

activity, which would have been encoded by strong connections from upstream

cells, suddenly become silent. For the system to restore its activity levels it

requires new upstream providers of activation, since the old ones have been

proven to no longer carry the information they used to. This could potentially be

achieved by amplifying every source of information in a correlated manner, as

proposed in the classical theory of synaptic scaling. Our results indicate however

that although scaling may occur when HSP occurs in an isolated system, when it

is embedded in a dynamic network undergoing different types of plasticity, more

complex dynamics may be at play. Firstly, the structure of the synapse, which

will be highly influential for the long-term consequences of the plasticity, does

not immediately scale in a correlated manner to the physiology. Instead, large

spines seem to respond initially to the activity blockade by increasing in volume,

with small spines following behind. Secondly, the sensitisation of small inputs to

LTP provides an alternative manner of increasing activity to pure synaptic

scaling. It primes the system for activation but only potentiates the spines that

Page 151: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

133

actually receive the stimulus. This is more efficient than increasing everything

equally, since in such a system further Hebbian plasticity would need to occur to

refine the circuit.

In Chapter 3, we expounded the idea that the small, highly sensitive spines we

saw after HSP could be the newly formed silent synapses, which are known to

arise after activity blockade in systems both in vivo and in vitro (Ashby and Isaac,

2011; Arendt et al., 2013). If it were verified that these silent synapses do have

an enhanced potential for plasticity, then this would have interesting

implications for the functioning of the downstream circuit, because it would

hasten the formation of an activity-dependent network. In the barrel cortex in

vivo, whisker trimming increased the proportion of silent synapses (Ashby and

Isaac, 2011). Interestingly, there was also a decoupling of structure and function,

i.e. between spine volume and conductance, as we observed in our own data

presented in Chapter 2. This mismatch was due specifically to the silent

synapses, because if only non-silent synapses were considered there was a

strong positive correlation between volume and conductance, a similar finding to

previous reports (Busetto et al., 2008). Ashby and Isaac suggest that the template

for connectivity in sensory cortex is experience independent, and is formed

through AMPAR-lacking spines. These are then unsilenced by activity which

drives AMPAR insertion. This mechanism, combined with our data that spines

after HSP have a decreased threshold for activity and increased magnitude of

LTP expression, emphasises the circuit-level sensitisation to any activity which

occurs after homeostasis. Although the mechanisms discussed in this thesis are

purely synaptic and nominally cell-autonomous, they will therefore also have

important implications for the network-level activity and organisation.

Page 152: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

134

4.4 Heterosynaptic plasticity and synaptic clustering

As well as the sensitisation of the spine receiving the activity, we also observed

structural plasticity in spines neighbouring it as presented in Figure 3.7 from

Chapter 3. An original tenet of Hebbian plasticity theory was that it is input-

specific, meaning the change in strength happens only at the synapse to which

the input has arrived. Many results in previous years have however challenged

this assumption, showing that plasticity at nearby synapses can affect each other

(Harvey and Svoboda, 2007; Govindarajan et al., 2011; Oh et al., 2015). This

phenomenon is known as heterosynaptic plasticity. It is intriguing that our

results point towards the co-option of heterosynaptic signalling occurring after

HSP, an association which has been hinted at in recent modelling studies

(Chistiakova et al., 2015).

One important mechanism of heterosynaptic plasticity which has received much

attention over the past decades is synaptic tagging and capture (Frey and Morris,

1997). In this process, a synapse is ‘tagged’ by receiving activity. This tagging

process, which may constitute a molecular or structural change to the synapse

(with rearrangements to the actin cytoskeleton being a possible candidate

(Fonseca, 2012)) allows it to capture plasticity-related proteins synthesised

nearby, at timeframes shortly preceding or succeeding the setting of the tag

(Redondo and Morris, 2011). Behaviourally, this will allow the binding together

of relevant stimuli in time to form better associations with events occurring in

the outside world. A second effect it will achieve is to sensitise the circuit to

activity, because stimuli which were previously sub-threshold or caused only

short-lasting plasticity are boosted, allowing them to cause long-lasting changes

(Govindarajan et al., 2011). This mechanism of sharing plasticity-related

resources is known as cooperation. Synapses can also compete for plasticity

when in a regime of limited resources (e.g. if protein synthesis is inhibited),

restricting the spatial and temporal range over which synapses can facilitate one

Page 153: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

135

another (Fonseca et al., 2004). On a structural level, the effect of synaptic

cooperation and competition can be profound for the organisation of synapses

on a dendrite. In particular, the sharing of plasticity-related proteins along a

limited synaptic distance will result in the formation of clusters of synapses

(Govindarajan et al., 2006; Ramiro-Cortés et al., 2014) (Figure 4.1 a – e). Cluster

formation will increase the memory storage capacity and computation power of

a circuit (Poirazi and Mel, 2001; Chklovskii et al., 2004). This effect is due to non-

linear dendritic integration mechanisms caused by voltage sensitive channels in

the membrane. These allow dendritic spiking, meaning inputs which arrive close

together in time and space are more effective at causing somatic depolarisation

Figure 4.1 Structural clustering after cooperation and competition

(adapted from Ramiro-Cortés, Hobbiss and Israely, 2014)

(a) A dendritic region receives a pattern of activity. Some spines receive strong late-LTP

inputs, in this example, spine i. Others (ii, iii, iv, v) receive weaker early-LTP inputs, which

set a synaptic tag. (b) Signalling mechanisms including protein synthesis are initiated

around spine i, which travel outwards along the dendrite. Tagged spines in the surrounding

regions (ii, iii, iv) compete for the resources. Spine v is outside the range of the proteins so

will not enter the competition. (c) The winning spines (i, ii, iii) are selectively strengthened.

The losing spine (iv) does not express long-lasting plasticity. At longer time scales: (d) new

spine growth can be initiated around the site of plasticity (dashed spines), while previously

existing spines that were not strengthened, are removed (faint spines). (e) A structural

cluster is formed, reflecting the summation of activity which came into the dendrite.

Page 154: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

136

(Branco and Häusser, 2009, 2011). There is emerging evidence for plasticity

clusters following Hebbian plasticity, implying that indeed this is a mechanism

that neurons employ to increase the capacity and stability of the circuit

(Kleindienst et al., 2011; Makino and Malinow, 2011; Fu et al., 2012; Lee et al.,

2016). The effect that synaptic clustering will have on HSP-mediated circuit

recovery following loss of input, and conversely that the expression of HSP will

have on the maintenance of pre-existing clusters, is still to be determined.

One likely effect supported by the data presented in this thesis is that

heterosynaptic mechanisms will cause a sensitisation of the circuit to new points

of activity. This could be an invaluable way to regain the requisite sources of

activation required to re-normalise network activity. If new synapses are formed

in response to activity blockade as is hinted at in previous data (Ashby and Isaac,

2011; Arendt et al., 2013), then these could be formed preferentially close to any

sites where activity still arrives (Figure 4.1 d). In a regime of global activity

blockade there will be no upstream activity so synaptogenesis may occur

randomly, which is why this effect may not have been reported in previous work.

However, in a more realistic system whereby some small amount of residual

activity remains, there could be a bias to form new synapses around these sites.

It is known that glutamate application alone can cause new synaptogenesis at

dendritic sites although this was discovered in an immature culture where

dendritic spines are as of yet unformed (Kwon and Sabatini, 2011). Since our

data points to a sensitisation of spines and dendritic sites to lower levels of

activation following HSP, it is plausible that any synaptic activation will cause

plasticity and synaptogenesis surrounding any sites of activity. The data

presented in Chapter 3 showing that small spines have an increased capacity for

potentiation would also enhance a clustering effect in this scenario, since the

newly formed small spines will be more easily potentiated and so will be

stabilised more easily. If HSP can indeed facilitate synaptic cluster formation, it

would accelerate the process of regaining the optimal activity level because the

Page 155: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

137

aforementioned non-linear dendritic integration would allow the cell to achieve

the same amount of output firing for less synaptic activation.

A tangible example of this type of circuit level change could be the re-wiring of

networks known to occur in the human cortex after a major perturbation, for

example going blind. The visual cortex is deprived of its major sources of input

and so its activity level will initially drop (Keck et al., 2013). Over time invasion

of the visual areas will occur from other sensory areas which still receive input

(Bavelier and Neville, 2002; Butz and van Ooyen, 2011). I will use the auditory

cortex as an example, although the somatosensory cortex is also commonly

involved (Dormal and Collignon, 2011; Lane et al., 2015). This new source of

innervations from auditory areas can result in increased auditory acuity due to a

larger neural space dedicated to it. Conversely loss of ‘real’ visual input can lead

to visual hallucinations (Reichert et al., 2013). These well-described phenomena

could be the result of the HSP-mediated re-wiring and cluster formation which

has been discussed (Butz and van Ooyen, 2013). It would fit the criteria of a large

activity change which can re-normalise not just by synaptic scaling, because the

previously reliable sources of upstream activity (in this case those carrying

visual information from the eyes) have become permanently silent. Instead, new

sources, those from the auditory areas which before were not important for

visual cortex functioning, suddenly become major players in providing synaptic

activation and so these synapses will become strengthened. Similarly, stroke-

induced lesions are known to induce neural remapping which could also be

accounted for by HSP mechanisms (Wittenberg, 2010; Butz et al., 2014).

4.5 Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity in health and disease

The main focus of this thesis has been to investigate how a ‘normal’ (i.e. wild-

type) system responds to perturbations of activity. The in vivo analogies which

Page 156: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

138

have been drawn are with systems suddenly losing input, such as in sensory

deprivation or limb loss. A separate field however has started to take great

interest in the ideas of homeostasis; that of neurological disorders. There is

emerging evidence that homeostatic processes in the brain might be

compromised in some common disorders. Studies of homeostatic processes in

these conditions give us information about the pathophysiology of the disease.

Additionally, we can also gain insight into the wild-type signalling processes by

studying what modifications (normally genetic mutations) interrupt

homeostasis, and therefore which signalling pathways and molecular players are

involved.

We began to address this theory in section 2.4.3, by investigating whether the

loss of the mTOR regulator Tsc1, which in humans causes the

neurodevelopmental disorder Tuberous Sclerosis, affected HSP. We found that

despite structural differences between Tsc1- and wild-type spines, with those

bearing the Tsc1 mutation being smaller, structural up-scaling was intact in this

model. It may be that other types of homeostatic plasticity, in particular synaptic

down-scaling, are impaired by loss of this signalling mechanism, since the

resultant circuit has pathologically high activity rates (Talos et al., 2012; Bateup

et al., 2013b). There are many other mental retardation disorders which have

been suggested to have aberrant homeostatic plasticity, including Fragile X

disorder (Soden and Chen, 2010; Sarti et al., 2013), Rett syndrome (Blackman et

al., 2012; Qiu et al., 2012; Zhong et al., 2012) and numerous others (reviewed in

(Wondolowski and Dickman, 2013)). Interestingly, what these diseases have in

common is a high propensity to autism – in fact, all of the aforementioned

diseases can be classified as autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) (Toro et al.,

2010). ASDs constitute a complex, heterogeneous set of developmental disorders

characterised by a set of clinical symptoms including deficits in social

behavioural and impaired communication skills (Newschaffer et al., 2007). One

promising line of investigation into the causes of ASDs has emerged from the

Page 157: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

139

findings that aberrant protein translation occurs frequently (Kelleher III and

Bear, 2008). Loss of normal levels of protein – or as we could say, loss of protein

homeostasis – causes diverse defects to synapses and cells, including changes to

spine density, spine volume, soma size and capacity for plasticity. Although

occurring in many different points in signalling pathways, there is a convergence

of the downstream effects with similar behavioural phenotypes being exhibited.

Since HSP relies on fine-tuning levels of receptors and other signalling molecules

through processes of transcription and translation, the disregulation of these

processes (either upwards or downwards) will have serious consequences for

the ability to correctly express homeostatic processes. It is interesting to

postulate that when homeostasis goes awry, it could result in not only the most

obvious disorder of pathological activity rates, i.e. epilepsy, but also in the

impairment of many other cognitive functions. The relevance of HSP to neural

functioning is not therefore limited to extreme cases such as limb loss or sensory

deprivation, but is vital throughout the whole lifetime of an animal and for the

maintenance of many if not all of the ‘normal’ processes taking place in the brain.

As well as neurodevelopmental disorders, other neurological diseases such as

Alzheimer’s Disease have been associated with loss of homeostasis, in this case

as a consequence of the ageing process (Jang and Chung, 2016). Furthermore,

neuropsychiatric disorders such as depression and schizophrenia are associated

with global structural and functional changes to cells and dendrites (Dickman

and Davis, 2009; Castrén and Hen, 2013). Chronic stress, a known risk factor for

developing depression, causes reduced dendritic length and spine density in

striatal neurons (Dias-Ferreira et al., 2009). These tantalising clues to aberrant

signalling and plasticity process may prove to be the first steps in learning that

homeostasis is vital for keeping the brain in a healthy state, and that a multitude

of different disorders can arise if it is disrupted.

Page 158: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

140

4.6 The role of sleep in homeostasis

The homeostatic processes so far referred to have been concurrent with the

normal functioning of the circuit. It has been proposed however that sleep, a

neural state found in a majority of animal species and characterised by loss of

consciousness and highly altered patterns of correlated neural activity, functions

as a specific time for the enactment of synaptic homeostasis. This theory, known

as the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis (SHY), proposes that during wakefulness

the majority of learning that takes place is through strengthening of excitatory

connections. The function of sleep, therefore, is to restore synaptic homeostasis

by allowing synaptic weakening (Tononi and Cirelli, 2003, 2014). The SHY

theory has been recently supported by experimental evidence (Liu et al., 2010;

Bushey et al., 2011; Grosmark et al., 2012), although some of its predictions and

assumptions have proved controversial (Frank, 2012; Durkin and Aton, 2015;

Hengen et al., 2016). As well as specific sleep/wake states, the circadian cycle has

been implicated in setting the timing of homeostatic synaptic changes

(Appelbaum et al., 2010).

From the point of view of the data presented in this thesis, the differentiation

between timeframes for Hebbian and homeostatic plasticity may help to

delineate which mechanism is promoted at a specific time (i.e. high calcium

causes LTP during wakefulness, and down-scaling during sleep). If this were

true, it would imply some further signalling, such as by neuromodulators, which

differs between the sleep-state and the wake-state and will promote one form of

plasticity over the other. In our reduced system, we lack neuromodulatory input

which may explain why homeostatic changes take so long (48 hours) to emerge,

whereas homeostatic processes are theorised to occur on much shorter

timescales in vivo (Zenke et al., 2013; Zenke and Gerstner, 2016). Many of the

same mechanisms are apparent however, which include structural modifications

and functional synaptic scaling (Liu et al., 2010; Bushey et al., 2011), suggesting

Page 159: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

141

that whilst the mechanisms of HSP may take longer to develop in our system, the

same pathways might be co-opted in the end.

In an interesting link to the previous section on health and disease, disrupted

sleep, and the ensuing implication of disrupted synaptic homeostasis, have been

explicitly implicated in serious mood disorders such as depression and suicidal

thoughts (Sher, 2009). If indeed sleep performs the vital role of promoting HSP

to counteract the plasticity from a day’s wakefulness, it is unsurprising that

serious psychological disorders could arise from lack of it, again reinforcing the

vital role homeostasis plays in maintaining our neural system in a healthy state.

4.7 Conclusion

Homeostatic mechanisms occurring at the synapse are induced by global shifts in

activity levels. These mechanisms encompass functional changes, due to receptor

changes at the synapse, and structural ones, likely to be the result of changes to

the cytoskeleton and other scaffolding molecules. The result is global changes to

synaptic strengths across the neuron in a correlated manner. The end result

however does not tally with a simplistic concept of synaptic scaling, since not

only do important structural components such as the volume not scale linearly,

but the sensitivity to future plasticity is modulated depending on spine size.

Therefore HSP will not just alter the activity level of the individual cell but by

adjusting the sensitivity to different inputs will allow the circuit to change shape

as well. This will be enhanced by metaplasticity mechanisms which are co-opted

at neighbouring synapses to allow them to express plasticity. The result could be

that new clusters of synapses will arise which carry correlated information, to

restore the cell to a functional position within the neural circuit. It would be

fascinating to understand in vivo whether new strengthening of small inputs

occurs and whether it does lead to circuit re-arrangement in this manner. If so

Page 160: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

142

this could a major mechanism for well-described homeostatic re-wiring

phenomena such as innervations of sensory cortices by other cortical areas after

sensory deprivation. Disruptions to HSP are also becoming increasingly

prominent as a cause for at least a subset of the cognitive impairments seen in

many neurodevelopmental and autism spectrum disorders.

These results emphasise the critical role HSP plays in maintaining neural circuits

in a healthy and high functioning state. Future work will serve to further

investigate these intriguing links and furnish our knowledge of the dynamics and

mechanisms of homeostatic synaptic plasticity.

Page 161: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

143

4.8 Bibliography

Appelbaum L, Wang G, Yokogawa T, Skariah GM, Smith SJ, Mourrain P, Mignot E (2010) Circadian and homeostatic regulation of structural synaptic plasticity in hypocretin neurons. Neuron 68:87–98.

Arendt KL, Sarti F, Chen L (2013) Chronic inactivation of a neural circuit enhances LTP by inducing silent synapse formation. J Neurosci 33:2087–2096.

Ashby MC, Isaac JTR (2011) Maturation of a Recurrent Excitatory Neocortical Circuit by Experience-Dependent Unsilencing of Newly Formed Dendritic Spines. Neuron 70:510–521.

Bateup HS, Johnson CA, Denefrio CL, Saulnier JL, Kornacker K, Sabatini BL (2013) Excitatory/inhibitory synaptic imbalance leads to hippocampal hyperexcitability in mouse models of tuberous sclerosis. Neuron 78:510–522.

Bavelier D, Neville HJ (2002) Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? Nat Rev Neurosci 3:443–452.

Blackman MP, Djukic B, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG (2012) A Critical and Cell-Autonomous Role for MeCP2 in Synaptic Scaling Up. J Neurosci 32:13529–13536.

Branco T, Häusser M (2009) The selfish spike: local and global resets of dendritic excitability. Neuron 61:815–817.

Branco T, Häusser M (2011) Synaptic integration gradients in single cortical pyramidal cell dendrites. Neuron 69:885–892.

Busetto G, Higley MJ, Sabatini BL (2008) Developmental presence and disappearance of postsynaptically silent synapses on dendritic spines of rat layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons. J Physiol 586:1519–1527.

Bushey D, Tononi G, Cirelli C (2011) Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: structural evidence in Drosophila. Science 332:1576–1581.

Butz M, Steenbuck ID, van Ooyen A (2014) Homeostatic structural plasticity can account for topology changes following deafferentation and focal stroke. Front Neuroanat 8:115.

Butz M, van Ooyen A (2011) Need for homeostasis in electrical activity may account for cortical network rewiring. BMC Neurosci 12:F1.

Page 162: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

144

Butz M, van Ooyen A (2013) A Simple Rule for Dendritic Spine and Axonal Bouton Formation Can Account for Cortical Reorganization after Focal Retinal Lesions. PLoS Comput Biol 9:39–43.

Castrén E, Hen R (2013) Neuronal plasticity and antidepressant actions. Trends Neurosci 36:259–267.

Chistiakova M, Bannon NM, Chen J-Y, Bazhenov M, Volgushev M (2015) Homeostatic role of heterosynaptic plasticity: models and experiments. Front Comput Neurosci 9:89.

Chklovskii D, Mel B, Svoboda K (2004) Cortical rewiring and information storage. Nature 431:782–788.

Dias-Ferreira E, Sousa JC, Melo I, Morgado P, Mesquita AR, Cerqueira JJ, Costa RM, Sousa N (2009) Chronic Stress Causes Frontostriatal Reorganization and Affects Decision-Making. Science (80- ) 325:621–625.

Dickman DK, Davis GW (2009) The Schizophrenia Susceptibility Gene Dysbindin Controls Synaptic Homeostasis. Science (80- ) 326:1127–1130.

Dormal G, Collignon O (2011) Functional selectivity in sensory-deprived cortices. J Neurophysiol 105:2627–2630.

Durkin J, Aton SJ (2015) Sleep-Dependent Potentiation in the Visual System Is at Odds with the Synaptic Homeostasis Hypothesis. Sleep 39:155–159.

Fonseca R (2012) Activity-dependent actin dynamics are required for the maintenance of long-term plasticity and for synaptic capture. Eur J Neurosci 35:195–206.

Fonseca R, Nägerl UV, Morris RGM, Bonhoeffer T (2004) Competing for memory: hippocampal LTP under regimes of reduced protein synthesis. Neuron 44:1011–1020.

Frank MG (2012) Erasing synapses in sleep: is it time to be SHY? Neural Plast 2012:264378.

Frey U, Morris R (1997) Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature.

Fu M, Yu X, Lu J, Zuo Y (2012) Repetitive motor learning induces coordinated formation of clustered dendritic spines in vivo. Nature 1:92–95.

Govindarajan A, Israely I, Huang S-Y, Tonegawa S (2011) The dendritic branch is the preferred integrative unit for protein synthesis-dependent LTP. Neuron 69:132–146.

Page 163: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

145

Govindarajan A, Kelleher RJ, Tonegawa S (2006) A clustered plasticity model of long-term memory engrams. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:575–583.

Grosmark AD, Mizuseki K, Pastalkova E, Diba K, Buzsáki G (2012) REM Sleep Reorganizes Hippocampal Excitability. Neuron 75:1001–1007.

Harvey CD, Svoboda K (2007) Locally dynamic synaptic learning rules in pyramidal neuron dendrites. Nature 450:1195–1200.

Hengen KB, Torrado Pacheco A, McGregor JN, Van Hooser SD, Turrigiano GG (2016) Neuronal Firing Rate Homeostasis Is Inhibited by Sleep and Promoted by Wake. Cell 165:1–12.

Jang S-S, Chung HJ (2016) Emerging Link between Alzheimer’s Disease and Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity. Neural Plast.

Keck T, Keller GB, Jacobsen RI, Eysel UT, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M (2013) Synaptic scaling and homeostatic plasticity in the mouse visual cortex in vivo. Neuron 80:327–334.

Kelleher III RJ, Bear MF (2008) The autistic neuron: troubled translation? Cell 135:401–406.

Kleindienst T, Winnubst J, Roth-Alpermann C, Bonhoeffer T, Lohmann C (2011) Activity-dependent clustering of functional synaptic inputs on developing hippocampal dendrites. Neuron 72:1012–1024.

Kwon H-B, Sabatini BL (2011) Glutamate induces de novo growth of functional spines in developing cortex - SI. Nature 474:100–104.

Lane C, Kanjlia S, Omaki A, Bedny M (2015) “Visual” Cortex of Congenitally Blind Adults Responds to Syntactic Movement. J Neurosci 35:12859–12868.

Lee KFH, Soares C, Thivierge J-P, Béïque J-C (2016) Correlated Synaptic Inputs Drive Dendritic Calcium Amplification and Cooperative Plasticity during Clustered Synapse Development. Neuron:1–16.

Liu Z-W, Faraguna U, Cirelli C, Tononi G, Gao X-B (2010) Direct Evidence for Wake-Related Increases and Sleep-Related Decreases in Synaptic Strength in Rodent Cortex. J Neurosci 30:8671–8675.

Makino H, Malinow R (2011) Compartmentalized versus Global Synaptic Plasticity on Dendrites Controlled by Experience. Neuron 72:1001–1011.

Newschaffer CJ, Croen LA, Daniels J, Giarelli E, Grether JK, Levy SE, Mandell DS, Miller L a, Pinto-Martin J, Reaven J, Reynolds AM, Rice CE, Schendel D,

Page 164: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

146

Windham GC (2007) The epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. Annu Rev Public Health 28:235–258.

Oh WC, Parajuli LK, Zito K (2015) Heterosynaptic Structural Plasticity on Local Dendritic Segments of Hippocampal CA1 Neurons. Cell Rep 10:162–169.

Poirazi P, Mel BW (2001) Impact of active dendrites and structural plasticity on the memory capacity of neural tissue. Neuron 29:779–796.

Qiu Z, Sylwestrak EL, Lieberman DN, Zhang Y, Liu X-Y, Ghosh A (2012) The Rett syndrome protein MeCP2 regulates synaptic scaling. J Neurosci 32:989–994.

Ramiro-Cortés Y, Hobbiss AF, Israely I (2014) Synaptic competition in structural plasticity and cognitive function. Philos Trans R Soc London Ser B, Biol Sci R … 369:20130157.

Redondo RL, Morris RGM (2011) Making memories last: the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:17–30.

Reichert DP, Seri??s P, Storkey AJ (2013) Charles Bonnet Syndrome: Evidence for a Generative Model in the Cortex? PLoS Comput Biol 9.

Sarti F, Zhang Z, Schroeder J, Chen L (2013) Rapid suppression of inhibitory synaptic transmission by retinoic acid. J Neurosci 33:11440–11450.

Sher L (2009) Do sleep abnormalities related to disruption of synaptic homeostasis contribute to the pathophysiology of post-traumatic mood disorder and suicidal behavior? Med Hypotheses 72:230.

Soden ME, Chen L (2010) Fragile X protein FMRP is required for homeostatic plasticity and regulation of synaptic strength by retinoic acid. J Neurosci 30:16910–16921.

Talos DM, Sun H, Zhou X, Fitzgerald EC, Jackson MC, Klein PM, Lan VJ, Joseph A, Jensen FE (2012) The interaction between early life epilepsy and autistic-like behavioral consequences: A role for the mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR) pathway. PLoS One 7:1–16.

Tononi G, Cirelli C (2003) Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: A hypothesis. Brain Res Bull 62:143–150.

Tononi G, Cirelli C (2014) Sleep and the Price of Plasticity: From Synaptic and Cellular Homeostasis to Memory Consolidation and Integration. Neuron 81:12–34.

Page 165: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

147

Toro R, Konyukh M, Delorme R, Leblond C, Chaste P, Fauchereau F, Coleman M, Leboyer M, Gillberg C, Bourgeron T (2010) Key role for gene dosage and synaptic homeostasis in autism spectrum disorders. Trends Genet 26:363–372.

Wittenberg GF (2010) Experience, cortical remapping, and recovery in brain disease. Neurobiol Dis 37:252–258.

Wondolowski J, Dickman D (2013) Emerging links between homeostatic synaptic plasticity and neurological disease. Front Cell Neurosci 7:223.

Zenke F, Gerstner W (2016) Cooperation across timescales between and Hebbian and homeostatic plasticity. :1–40.

Zenke F, Hennequin G, Gerstner W (2013) Synaptic Plasticity in Neural Networks Needs Homeostasis with a Fast Rate Detector. PLoS Comput Biol 9.

Zhong X, Li H, Chang Q (2012) MeCP2 Phosphorylation Is Required for Modulating Synaptic Scaling through mGluR5. J Neurosci 32:12841–12847.

Page 166: Anna Felicity Hobbiss - run.unl.pt Hobbiss Thesis.pdf · Anna Felicity Hobbiss Research work coordinated by: Oeiras, 12th July, 2016 modulation of dendritic spines driven by homeostatic

148

Neuronal Structure and Function lab, April 2016

Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, June 2012

Neuronal Structure and Function lab, April 2016